Поиск по сайту




Пишите нам: info@ethology.ru

Follow etholog on Twitter

Система Orphus

Новости
Библиотека
Видео
Разное
Кросс-культурный метод
Старые форумы
Рекомендуем
Не в тему

список статей


Sex differences in children's investment in same-sex peers

Joyce F Benensona, Kiran Alavib

1. Introduction

2. Method

2.1. Participants

2.2. Procedure

3. Results

4. Discussion

References

Copyright

1. Introduction

Universally, human mothers invest more than fathers in offspring (for reviews, see Brown, 1991, Hewlett, 1992) as is typical across mammalian species Krasnegor & Bridges, 1990, Trivers, 1972. Little is known however about sex differences in investment in unrelated individuals (Hill, 2002). This gap likely exists because of the complexity involved in defining and measuring investment between unrelated individuals.

In studies of human beings' closest living genetic relatives, chimpanzees, grooming, proximity, and food sharing are used to measure investment. Results indicate that unrelated adult males are more strongly bonded than unrelated adult females in common chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, whereas the reverse is true for bonobos, Pan paniscusde Waal, 2001, White & Chapman, 1995, Wrangham, 1986. In studies of humans in traditional societies with few resources, food sharing commonly serves as a measure of investment (for a review, see Gurven, in press). In societies where food is relatively plentiful however, other measures are required.

The current study was designed to measure sex differences in investment between unrelated same-sex children using durability of bonds as the measure of investment. As a measure of investment, durability permits assessment over relatively long time intervals and is less influenced than proximity or food sharing by immediate external forces, such as familial or educational constraints. Whereas long distance relationships between adults undermine the validity of durability as a measure of continued investment (Rodseth & Wrangham, in press), durability is useful as a measure for children whose relationships with unrelated individuals are predominantly local.

Several lines of evidence indicate that beginning in middle childhood in numerous and diverse cultures, males invest more than females in unrelated same-sex individuals. Firstly, boys exhibit greater enjoyment and spend more time with same-sex peers than girls do (Benenson et al., 1998, Bryant, 1985, Whiting & Edwards, 1988; for reviews, see Maccoby, 1998, Schlegel & Barry, 1991). Secondly, boys interact in larger groups of same-sex peers than girls who prefer to form a close bond with only one peer at a time (for reviews, see Belle, 1989, Cairns et al., 1998, Schlegel & Barry, 1991), a pattern that continues into adulthood Baumeister & Sommers, 1997, Brown, 1991, Gabriel & Gardner, 1999, Seeley et al., 2003, Tiger, 1969. The larger number of same-sex partners with whom males interact suggests that the cost–benefit ratio of associating with others of the same sex is lower for males than females (Wrangham, 2000).

Finally, two prior studies with preadolescents and adolescents demonstrate that the durability of close same-sex friendships is greater for males than females (Benenson & Christakos, 2003, Kon & Losenkov, 1978), despite females' greater preference for this form of relationship (Belle, 1989). Analyzing this form of relationship thus constitutes a conservative test of the hypothesis that males' relationships with unrelated same-sex peers are more durable than those of females.

Nevertheless, these latter studies suffer from several limitations. First, they were based on self-reports. Cross-cultural evidence indicates that females are more likely than males to provide negative self-evaluations (for a review, see Kling, Hyde, Showers, & Bushwell, 1999), which may have contributed to the sex difference. Second, both studies included only preadolescents and adolescents. During this period, sex differences in rates of maturation translate into females' confronting the challenges introduced by sexual maturation before males do (Tanner, 1962). Consequently, competition between same-sex individuals for mating partners occurs earlier for females than males (Campbell, 1995), which may have weakened females' friendships. Third, the prior studies ignored the availability of resources (Kon & Losenkov, 1978) or included only individuals with few resources (Benenson & Christakos, 2003). Research with chimpanzees indicates that females invest more heavily in same-sex relationships when resources are plentiful (Boesch & Boesch-Achermann, 2000, de Waal, 1982; see Wrangham, 2000, for a theoretical discussion of feeding competition in higher primates and its influence on female bonding). Belle (1987) similarly finds that human females with few resources experience difficulties sustaining relationships with unrelated females. Belle concludes that economically stressed mothers must invest in offspring and cannot afford to share resources with unrelated females.

In order to overcome these limitations, in this study external observers assessed the durability of same-sex friendships in children who had not yet entered puberty. In addition, participants were selected because they came from economically advantaged backgrounds to ensure that they had access to plentiful resources. The goal was to test whether under these conditions relationships between males still would be more durable than those between females. As a second goal, the dynamics of conflicts between current close same-sex friends were compared for males and females. If sex differences were found in the number or quality of conflicts with same-sex friends, these could illuminate the proximate mechanisms that produce sex differences in investment in unrelated same-sex individuals.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

Seventeen classes from grades 1 to 6 in a private school in Montreal, Canada, participated in the study. On average, there were significantly more boys (M=14.65, S.D.=1.77, range 10–17) than girls (M=11.94, S.D.=1.39, range 10–15), t(32)=4.96, p<.001, in a class. Given that class sizes were large however, each sex was believed to have had ample opportunity to select compatible same-sex friends. Within each class, three to four children of each sex were interviewed about the friendships in their classes following the procedure of Lagerspetz, Bjorkqvist, and Peltonen (1988). Participants were chosen randomly. Consent forms were sent home with students in a class until at least three children of each sex had obtained parental consent to participate and were willing to be interviewed. Participants included 107 children (54 boys and 53 girls) ranging in age from 6 to 11 years. Ages for each sex and grade level are presented in Table 1. Over 95% of children who participated were Caucasian and from upper–middle class socioeconomic class backgrounds.

Table 1.

Mean ages of children

Grade level Females Males
M S.D. n M S.D. n
1 6.67 0.52 6 6.83 0.41 6
2 7.90 0.32 10 7.80 0.42 10
3 8.67 0.50 9 8.33 0.50 9
4 9.89 0.33 9 9.44 0.53 9
5 10.33 0.50 9 10.40 0.53 10
6 11.40 0.52 10 11.20 0.44 10

2.2. Procedure

To ensure that children had had time to establish friendships, data collection occurred during the months of April and May so that children had been in class together for over 6 months. All participants were interviewed individually by a female experimenter (the second author) who explained to each participant that she or he was going to be asked questions about friendships in the child's class. The interview consisted of two parts. In the first part, individuals were asked to name a pair of current same-sex friends in their classroom and to describe how these friends managed conflicts. In the second part, individuals were asked whether there were any same-sex friendships in the class that had ended.

In the first part of the questionnaire, each participant first was asked to name two children who were friends and were the same sex as the participant. Participants were instructed to refer only to the members of the friendship pair they had named when responding to the next four questions that concerned the frequency and duration of conflicts as well as the immediate and long-term levels of distress experienced following conflicts.

The question on frequency was phrased “How often do (Friend 1) and (Friend 2) have a conflict, disagreement, or fight about something?” and the response scale ranged from 1=less than once a week to 5=several times a day. The question regarding duration stated “After they have a conflict, disagreement, or fight, how long is it before (Friend 1) and (Friend 2) are friends again?” followed by a scale ranging from 1=within a few minutes to 5=several days or more. Next, participants were asked to ascertain the friends' immediate level of emotional distress following a conflict, “How angry or sad does (Friend 1) become when (Friend 1) and (Friend 2) have a conflict, disagreement, or fight?” The question was repeated for Friend 2 with both questions followed by a scale ranging from 1=not particularly angry or sad to 5=terribly angry or sad. Responses for each friend were averaged. The final question on levels of long-term distress stated, “Once the conflict, disagreement, or fight between (Friend 1) and (Friend 2) ends, are there any signs that either of them still feels bad about it? For example, do they talk to the other kids about the fight?” and the scale consisted of 1=no signs of feeling bad; once the conflict, disagreement, or fight is over, it's forgotten to 5=many signs of feeling bad; it's clear they have not forgotten about it.

In the second part of the questionnaire, participants were asked, “Can you name two boys/girls in your class who used to be friends but are no longer friends?” Again, participants were asked only about individuals in their class of their own sex. If participants answered affirmatively, they then were asked for the first names of the pair of children in their class who used to be friends. Each participant who named one pair of friends who no longer were friends with one another was then asked, “Are there any other boys/girls in your class who used to be friends but are no longer friends?” This question was repeated until children could not name any more friendships that had ended.

3. Results

To ensure statistical independence, the class constituted the unit of analysis. Within each class consequently, data from the three to four participants of each sex were averaged to generate one boy's score and one girl's score per class. To ensure an adequate sample size, data were collapsed from grades 1 to 2, 3 to 4, and 5 to 6 producing three grade levels. There were five classes from grades 1 to 2 (two from grade 1 and three from grade 2), six classes from grades 3 to 4 (three from each grade level), and six classes from grades 5 to 6 (three from each grade level). An analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted for each variable, with sex and grade as the independent variables.

No sex differences were found in reports of either frequency (M=1.94 for males; M=1.76 for females, F<1), nor duration (M=2.43 for males; M=2.04 for females, F(1,28)=1.93) of conflicts between current same-sex friends. Neither the effects of grade (F(2,28)=2.09, n.s. for frequency and F<1 for duration) nor the Sex × Grade interactions (F<1 for frequency and F(2,28)=1.39, n.s. for duration) were significant.

Likewise, no sex differences were obtained in reports of levels of immediate emotional distress (M=2.60 for males; M=2.83 for females, F(1,28)=1.09, n.s.) nor long-term emotional distress (M=1.44 for males; M=1.49 for females, F<1) following conflicts between current same-sex friends. There was a significant effect of grade for level of immediate distress, F(2,28)=4.47, p<.025. Tukey's test, p<.05, revealed that compared to children in both older grade levels (M=2.57 for grades 3–4; M=2.44 for grades 5–6), the youngest children (M=3.20) reported that conflicts generated greater emotional distress. The remaining effects were all nonsignificant, Fs<1.

Nonetheless, participants reported significantly more same-sex friendships had ended between girls than between boys, F(1,28)=8.88, p=.006. Neither the main effects of grade, F<1, nor the interaction between sex and grade, F(2,34)=1.48, n.s., was significant. Table 2 displays the means and standard deviations for past friendships within the three grade levels.

Table 2.

Mean number of same-sex friendships that had ended

Grade level Number of classes Females Males
n M S.D. M S.D.
1–2 5 1.02 0.96 0.22 0.33
3–4 6 0.61 0.25 0.50 0.28
5–6 6 0.85 0.45 0.26 0.39

A more stringent analysis then was conducted in which only the total number of different same-sex friendships that had ended in each class was included. That is, if two or more children of the same sex in a class named the same friendship that had ended, that friendship was included only once in calculating the total score for the children of that sex in that class. Using a matched-pair t test, the results remained unchanged, t(15)=3.11, p=.007 (females: M=2.47, S.D.=1.80; range 1–7 past friendships per class; males: M=1.06, S.D.=1.08; range 0–3 past friendships per class).

Finally, the number of males and females who named at least one past same-sex friendship in the class that had ended was compared. Over twice as many females (56.60% or 30/53) as males (27.77% or 15/54) named at least one same-sex friendship in their classes that had ended, χ2=9.12, p=.003.

4. Discussion

Results demonstrate that beginning as early as 6 years of age, even amongst young children with access to abundant resources, external observers report that fewer males' than females' same-sex friendships had ended. Given that the study examined females' preferred form of relationship, dyadic friendships, the findings represent a stringent test of the hypothesis that bonds between males are more durable than those between females. Interestingly, although participants were not asked about other-sex friendships, five boys across the grade levels spontaneously reported that one or more friendships between girls in their class that had ended. By contrast, only one girl in the first grade reported that a pair of boys were no longer friends.

It has been well established that human mothers maintain more durable ties than fathers to offspring (for reviews, see Brown, 1991, Hewlett, 1992). It is extremely rare for a mother to terminate a relationship with her child, whereas fathers vary widely in their degree of parental investment. Researchers therefore often assume that the same biological mechanisms that ensure greater investment by mothers than fathers in kin extend to other forms of relationships (Taylor et al., 2000, but see Geary & Flinn, 2002). Results from the current study suggest that sex differences in degree of parental investment do not necessarily translate into sex differences in investment in unrelated same-sex individuals.

Knowledge of the ecological costs and benefits that accrue from mothers' investing in unrelated same-sex individuals may provide clues as to reasons for this sex difference. To illustrate, research with chimpanzees indicates that sociability for mothers with dependent offspring relates to degree of scramble competition over resources, particularly food (Wrangham, 2000). When food is abundant, females can afford to associate with other unrelated females who may provide benefits such as protection from males and other predators (Boesch & Boesch-Achermann, 2000) or opportunities for infants and juveniles to play (Goodall, 1986). In common chimpanzees, high status females who are better able to obtain food (Pusey, Williams, & Goodall, 1997) also are more likely to affiliate with one another Boesch & Boesch-Achermann, 2000, de Waal, 1982. When food is scarce however or when females are low status, they may avoid other unrelated females to conserve resources for themselves and their offspring. Whether the same algorithms evolved in human females or the same ecological factors apply to modern humans however remains unknown. Nevertheless, it is reasonable to hypothesize that the uniquely human costs of simultaneously provisioning multiple dependent offspring (Lancaster, 1991) may discourage females from investing too heavily in another individual who confronts the same costs and as such cannot supply surplus resources.

Relative to females, males may benefit more at present or have gained more in the ancestral past from maintaining bonds with other males. de Waal (1989) suggests that chimpanzee males who require alliances in order to increase their dominance rank have evolved to reconcile conflicts rapidly. In contrast, chimpanzee females who depend far less on other chimpanzees have not evolved such mechanisms for reconciliation mechanisms. Rodseth and Wrangham (2004) further propose that alliances are more important for males than females because territorial politics and ultimately warfare are predominantly male domains.

The current study fails to provide behavioral evidence for sex differences in number or duration of conflict or levels of short- or long-term distress generated by conflict in current same-sex friendships. Possibly external observers were unaware of conflicts between children who currently were close friends. Suggestive findings from past studies of children's and adolescents' peer relationships indicate that compared to males, females report that they have been more hurt by their current closest same-sex friends (Benenson & Christakos, 2003) and that they are more likely to remain angry after a conflict (e.g., Lagerspetz et al., 1988). Further, observers of young children find that females are less likely than males to utilize joking to repair relationships (Butovskaya, Verbeek, Ljungvery, & Lunardini, 2000). These findings merit replication.

The current research cannot be generalized to adults with offspring. No research has compared the durability of same-sex bonds between adult males versus females. One difficulty is that in modern societies, adult relationships can be maintained at large distances with infrequent contact (Rodseth & Wrangham, 2004), so that it is unclear how to define and measure investment in a relationship. Self-sacrifice or cooperation constitute potentially valuable definitions of investment. Analogous data on adults must be collected before conclusions can be drawn regarding the evolutionary origins of sex differences in investment in unrelated individuals.

In summary, the current findings indicate that as early as 6 years of age, males form more durable bonds than females with unrelated same-sex individuals. If the same results are replicated with additional measures of investment and the findings extended to adults, this suggests that in human beings, investment in same-sex relationships exacts a higher cost for females relative to males or alternatively provides a larger benefit to males. Knowledge of ancestral and current ecological conditions will be necessary to provide a full evolutionary account of the basis for this sex difference.

References

Baumeister & Sommers, 1997 1.Baumeister RF, Sommers KL. What do men want? Gender differences and two spheres of belongingness: comment on Cross and Madson (1997). Psychological Bulletin. 1997;122:38–44. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Belle, 1987 2.Belle D. Gender differences in the social moderators of stress. In:  Barnett R,  Biener L,  Baruch G editor. Gender and stress. New York: Free Press; 1987;p. 257–277.

Belle, 1989 3.Belle D. Gender differences in children's social networks and supports. In:  Belle D editors. Children's social networks and social supports. New York: Wiley; 1989;p. 173–188.

Benenson & Christakos, 2003 4.Benenson JF, Christakos A. The greater fragility of females' versus males' closest same-sex friendships. Child Development. 2003;74:1123–1129. MEDLINE

Benenson et al., 1998 5.Benenson JF, Morganstein T, Roy R. Sex differences in children's investment in peers. Human Nature. 1998;9:369–390.

Butovskaya et al., 2000 6.Butovskaya M, Verbeek P, Ljungberg T, Lunardini A. A multicultural view of peacemaking among young children. In:  Aureli F,  de Waal FBM editor. Natural conflict resolution. London: University of California Press; 2000;p. 243–252.

Boesch & Boesch-Achermann, 2000 7.Boesch C, Boesch-Achermann H. The chimpanzees of the Tai forest. New York: Oxford University Press; 2000;.

Brown, 1991 8.Brown DE. Human universals. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1991;.

Bryant, 1985 9.Bryant BK. The neighborhood walk: sources of support in middle childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1985;50(3):.

Cairns et al., 1998 10.Cairns R, Xie H, Leung M. The popularity of friendship and the neglect of social networks: toward a new balance. In:  Bukowski WM,  Cillessen AH editor. Sociometry then and now: building on six decades of measuring children's experiences with the peer group. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1998;p. 25–54.

Campbell, 1995 11.Campbell A. A few good men: evolutionary psychology and female adolescent aggression. Ethology and Sociobiology. 1995;16:99–123.

de Waal, 1982 12.de Waal FBM. Chimpanzee politics. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press; 1982;.

de Waal, 1989 13.de Waal FBM. Peacemaking among primates. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1989;.

de Waal, 2001 14.de Waal FBM. Apes from Venus: bonobos and human social evolution. In:  de Waal FBM editors. Tree of origin. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2001;p. 39–68.

Gabriel & Gardner, 1999 15.Gabriel S, Gardner WL. Are there “his” and “hers” types of interdependence? The implications of gender differences in collective versus relational interdependence for affect, behavior, and cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1999;77:642–655. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Geary & Flinn, 2002 16.Geary DC, Flinn MV. Sex differences in behavioural and hormonal response to social threat: commentary on Taylor et al. (2000). Psychological Review. 2002;109:745–750. CrossRef

Goodall, 1986 17.Goodall J. The chimpanzees of Gombe. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1986;.

Gurven, in press 18.Gurven, M. (in press). To give and to give not: the behavioral ecology of human food transfers. Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

Hewlett, 1992 19.In:  Hewlett BS editors. Father–child relations. New York: Aldine de Gruyter; 1992;.

Hill, 2002 20.Hill K. Altruistic cooperation during foraging by the Ache, and the evolved human predisposition to cooperate. Human Nature. 2002;13:105–128.

Kling et al., 1999 21.Kling KC, Hyde JS, Showers CJ, Buswell BN. Gender differences in self-esteem: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 1999;125:470–500. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Kon & Losenkov, 1978 22.Kon IS, Losenkov VA. Friendship in adolescence: values and behavior. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1978;40:143–155.

Krasnegor & Bridges, 1990 23.In:  Krasnegor NA,  Bridges RS editor. Mammalian parenting. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1990;.

Lagerspetz et al., 1988 24.Lagerspetz KMJ, Bjorkqvist K, Peltonen T. Is indirect aggression typical of females? Gender differences in aggressiveness in 11- to 12-year-old children. Aggressive Behavior. 1988;14:303–315.

Lancaster, 1991 25.Lancaster JB. A feminist and evolutionary biologist looks at women. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology. 1991;34:1–11.

Maccoby, 1998 26.Maccoby EE. The two sexes: growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1998;.

Pusey et al., 1997 27.Pusey A, Williams J, Goodall J. The influence of dominance rank on the reproductive success of female chimpanzees. Science. 1997;277:828–831. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Rodseth & Wrangham, 2004 28.Rodseth L, Wrangham RW. Human kinship: a continuation of politics by other means?. In:  Chapais B,  Berman C editor. Kinship and behavior in primates. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004;.

Schlegel & Barry, 1991 29.Schlegel A, Barry H. Adolescence: an anthropological inquiry. New York: Free Press; 1991;.

Seeley et al., 2003 30.Seeley EA, Gardner WL, Pennington G, Gabriel S. Circle of friends or members of a group? Sex differences in relational and collective attachment to groups. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations. 2003;6:251–263.

Tanner, 1962 31.Tanner JM. Growth at adolescence. New York: Lippincott; 1962;.

Taylor et al., 2000 32.Taylor SE, Klein LC, Lewis BP, Gruenewald TL, Gurung RAR, Updegraff JA. Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review. 2000;107:411–429. CrossRef

Tiger, 1969 33.Tiger L. Men in groups. New York: Vintage Books; 1969;.

Trivers, 1972 34.Trivers R. Parental investment and sexual selection. In:  Campbell B editors. Sexual selection and the descent of man. Chicago: Aldine; 1972;p. 136–179.

White & Chapman, 1995 35.White FJ, Chapman CA. Contrasting chimpanzees and bonobos: nearest neighbour distances and choices. Folia Primatologica. 1995;63:181–191.

Whiting & Edwards, 1988 36.Whiting BB, Edwards CP. Children of different worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1988;.

Wrangham, 1986 37.Wrangham RW. Ecology and social relationships in two species of chimpanzee. In:  Rubenstein D,  Wrangham R editor. Ecological aspects of social evolution: birds and mammals. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1986;p. 352–378.

Wrangham, 2000 38.Wrangham R. Why are male chimpanzees more gregarious than mothers? A scramble competition hypothesis. In:  Kappeler PM editors. Primate males: causes and consequences of variation in group composition. Cambridge: University of Cambridge Press; 2000;p. 248–258.

a Department of Psychology, University of Plymouth, Drake Circus, Plymouth, Devon, PL4 8AA, UK

b Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada

Corresponding author. Tel.: +44-01752-233157; fax: +44-01752-233362

PII: S1090-5138(04)00038-8

doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.05.002



2007:11:13