Поиск по сайту




Пишите нам: info@ethology.ru

Follow etholog on Twitter

Система Orphus

Новости
Библиотека
Видео
Разное
Кросс-культурный метод
Старые форумы
Рекомендуем
Не в тему

список статей


Genetic relatedness and sibling relationship characteristics in a modern society

Thomas V. Pollet

1. Introduction

2. Methodology

3. Results

3.1. Assumption

3.2. Analysis 1: concern given and received over the past 3 months

3.3. Analysis 2: concern given and received over the past 3 months (including missing)

3.4. Analysis 3: face-to-face contact over the past 12 months

4. Discussion

5. Conclusion

Acknowledgment

References

Copyright

1. Introduction

Recently, individualization theorists, for example, Giddens (1991) and Beck (1993), have argued that social and family relationships have become increasingly dependent on individual choice rather than traditional norms and structures. According to this view, genetic relatedness only plays a minor role for kin and family relationships in modern societies. Although some theorists acknowledge that members of ‘patchwork’ families might have to work harder to overcome familial problems (Beck-Gernsheim, 1998), it is implicitly and sometimes explicitly assumed that genetic relatedness plays virtually no role for modern family relationships.

Evolutionists, by contrast, predict that genetic relatedness will affect family relationships in both traditional and modern societies (Hamilton, 1964, Emlen, 1995, Emlen, 1997).

Anthropological studies have shown that in traditional societies, closer related kin more often provide (mutual) material and immaterial support than distantly related kin, all else being equal. Closer related individuals are generally more likely to form coalitions for conflicts than distantly or unrelated individuals (Yanomamö: Chagnon & Bugos, 1979; Norse Earldoms, Icelandic families and English royalty: Johnson & Johnson, 1991; Viking Sagas: Dunbar, Clark, & Hurst, 1995). Besides forming coalitions for conflict, closely related kin often cooperate to overcome what is known as ‘the dilemma of collective action’ (Olson, 1965). Kin in traditional societies often help each other without requiring a direct return for the given help to a relative. By contrast, help from unrelated individuals appears to be based on direct reciprocity. This has been shown for agricultural tasks, for example, among the Ye'kwana of Venezuela (Hames, 1987) and the K'ekchi of Belize (Berté, 1988), and for hunting (Inuit: Morgan, 1979; but see Alvard, 2003). Closely related kin also often share meat as well as other food items (Cashdan, 1985, Betzig & Turke, 1986, Gurven et al., 2000, Gurven, 2004). An analysis of worker remittances in a modern society showed that closely related kin are more likely to receive money than distantly related kin (Bowles & Posel, 2005). In addition, individuals appear more willing to incur significant ‘costs’ for closely related individuals than for distantly related or unrelated individuals (Burnstein et al., 1994, Madsen et al., in press). However, this effect of relatedness on altruistic behavior appears to be mediated by emotional closeness (Korchmaros & Kenny, 2001). Incurring costs to aid closely related kin, rather than distantly related or unrelated individuals, is also more likely to be deemed rational and ethical (Kruger, 2001). Kinship cues, sharing the same last name (Oates & Wilson, 2002) for example, have been shown to facilitate altruism. Other kinship cues such as olfactory cues (Wedekind & Furi, 1997), facial resemblance (DeBruine, 2002, DeBruine, 2004a, DeBruine, 2004b, DeBruine, 2005), attitude similarity (Park & Schaller, 2005), and co-residence (Fessler & Navarrete, 2004, Lieberman et al., 2003) thoroughly affect the evaluation of and interaction between individuals.

Kin also make up a significant proportion of one's social network (Dunbar & Spoors, 1995, Hill & Dunbar, 2003). In traditional societies, closer related individuals have been shown to interact more often with each other than distantly related kin (Hames, 1979). Similarly, genetic relatedness is a significant predictor of subjective closeness and social support in modern societies (Neyer & Lang, 2003). The important role of kin for social support (e.g., Miller & Darlington, 2002, Kana'Iaupuni et al., 2005) could lead to inclusive fitness benefits, even in modern societies.

Thus, studies from a kin selection perspective show that biological kinship ties can heavily influence cooperation and (mutual) investments between individuals in terms of material and social support. However, relatively few studies have examined the effect of genetic relatedness on sibling relationships, especially in contemporary societies. While there has been considerable research on parent–offspring relations in patchwork families (Daly & Wilson, 1981, Daly & Wilson, 1982, Daly & Wilson, 1985, Daly & Wilson, 1988, Cherlin & Furstenberg, 1994, Stewart, 2005, Zvoch, 1999), the study of sibling relationships within these families has been largely neglected. Evolutionary analyses of sibling relationships so far have mainly focused on sibling differentiation (Dunn & Plomin, 1991, Plomin & Daniels, 1987), sibling rivalry (Sulloway, 1996, Sulloway, 2001), and incest avoidance (Aoki, 2005, Bevc & Silverman, 1993, Bevc & Silverman, 2000, Fessler & Navarrete, 2004, Lieberman et al., 2003).

Emlen (1997) suggested that individuals will invest more in full siblings (FS) than in half-siblings or stepsiblings. Sibling ties are predicted to be weaker and display more conflict when siblings are not fully related. In line with this prediction, parents and children report more conflict when siblings are not fully related than when they are fully related (Hetherington, 1988, Aquilino, 1991 for a review). Jankowiak & Diderich (2000) found that, despite a strong cultural ideology for equal treatment of half-siblings, half-siblings displayed significantly lower solidarity than FS in a polygamous Mormon community.

However, besides genetic relatedness, social factors, such as age or gender, heavily determine relationship dynamics between adult siblings (White & Riedmann, 1992). Research has shown that women act as kin keepers (Fischer, 1982, Lee et al., 1990, Rosenthal, 1985, White & Riedmann, 1992). Therefore, women are predicted to have stronger relationships with their siblings. Similarity should also influence relationship strength: the more similar the individuals in a sibling dyad are, the stronger their relationship is predicted to be (see McPherson, Smith-Lovin, & Cook, 2001). Following individualization theory, higher educated individuals are predicted to be less concerned with their sibling than lower educated individuals (see Beck & Beck-Gernsheim, 2002). Following exchange theory (Homans, 1951, Homans, 1958), financial relationships between siblings should also mirror social relationships. Balanced relationships and relationships without conflicts should be stronger and show more social investments (see Blau, 1964, but see Molm, 1997).

Predictions from these theories are not at odds with an evolutionary analysis of sibling ties, but are necessary as they address other aspects of sibling relationships. My analysis focuses on one aspect of sibling relationships (Jankowiak & Diderich, 2000), namely, social investments by siblings in each other. Social investment refers to keeping in contact and showing concern. Showing concern is necessary to assess the state of the sibling relationship. Although the costs of these social investments are probably not very high, they do represent a cost in terms of time and also an opportunity cost. Moreover, these social investments reflect relationship strength, and possibly a willingness to incur costs.

My hypothesis is that evolved psychological mechanisms regulating nepotistic investment independently affect the social investments made by siblings. Sibling dyads where the siblings are not fully related are predicted to display lower rates of social investment in one another. No study so far has investigated the independent effect of psychological mechanisms governing nepotistic investment on the sibling relationship while controlling for social factors.

There is however an alternative hypothesis, namely, that growing up together is the only important factor for the strength of the sibling dyad and that other evolved psychological mechanisms, such as third-party knowledge, play no role. Two distinct perspectives lead to this hypothesis. Identification theory (Stets & Burke, 2000) would lead to predict that growing up together increases sibling dyad strength because individuals will be more likely to categorize themselves as family members and behave accordingly. A kin recognition perspective suggests that propinquity during childhood is an important cue for identifying and interacting with kin (Fessler & Navarrete, 2004, Lieberman et al., 2003). I deal with this alternative hypothesis in two ways. Firstly, I control for age difference. The larger the age difference, the less likely that siblings will have shared childhood experiences. Secondly, I will compare maternal and paternal half-siblings (PHS). Children are more likely to remain with their mother if parents separate (De Graaf, 1997). Therefore, maternal half-siblings (MHS) are more likely to have been raised together than PHS. If differences with fully related siblings are found, not only for PHS but also for MHS, I can conclude that a psychological mechanism besides childhood propinquity influences sibling ties. Thus, if while controlling for age difference, I find a consistent difference between MHS and FS, this is reasonably strong evidence for the existence of another psychological mechanism regulating investment in kin rather than shared childhood experiences.

2. Methodology

The first wave of the Netherlands Kinship Panel Study (NKPS) data set was obtained through the Netherlands Interdisciplinary Demographic Institute. The NKPS is a large-scale longitudinal study, designed to investigate family and kin relations in the Netherlands (Dykstra et al., 2004). The first wave was completed mid-2004; I used the most recent version of the first-wave data set for this study (version of 21-7-05; main sample). The sampling procedure, representativity, as well as the survey method are described in detail by Dykstra et al.

In the NKPS, questions were asked about ‘sibling A’, a randomly selected sibling of the respondent (n=7544). Sibling A was a full, half-, or adopted sibling of the respondent.1 As in only 19 cases, sibling A was an adopted sibling; this category was excluded from the analyses. Therefore, the analyses include three categories for sibling type, fully related (n=7265), PHS (n=134), and MHS (n=126). In order to test the assumption that children are more likely to live with their mother than with their father, I will analyze all family transitions as reported by respondents until they were 16 years old.

Correspondent characteristics of sibling A, for example, gender, were associated by use of the sibling identification code (see Dykstra et al., 2004). The variables selected for analyses are presented in Table 1. The correspondent questions and additional information can be found in the NKPS codebook (Dykstra et al., 2004), except for the constructed variables (age difference, education difference, gender of sibling A, total living siblings, sibling type, education difference). Age difference was calculated as the absolute difference in birth years between siblings; it is therefore an approximation. Education difference is the absolute difference between the respondent and sibling A in educational attainment categories, with a high score indicating a strong dissimilarity.

Table 1.

Variables, categories, and predictions

Variables Categories Prediction
Educational attainment of respondent1,2,3 Eight categories (from incomplete/primary to university/postgraduate) Less contact if higher educated
Difference in educational attainment between siblings1,2,3 Seven categories (treated as interval) Less contact if large difference
Age difference between siblings1,2,3 Interval (difference in birth years) Less contact if large difference
Age respondent (at interview)1,2,3 Interval Less contact if older
Gender of the respondent1,2,3 0=male; 1=female More contact if female
Gender of sibling A1,2,3 0=male; 1=female More contact if female
Living full, half-, and adopted siblings1,2,3 Interval Less contact if more siblings
Sibling type1,2,3 1=PHS More contact if fully related, MHS will have more contact than PHS
2=MHS
3=FS
Financial balance1 1=respondent gives more; 2=other gives more More contact if balanced
3=balanced
Financial help given1 0=yes; 1=no More concern if money given
Financial help received1 0=yes; 1=no More concern if money received
Initiative contact 1 1=usually my initiative; 2=usually other's More contact if balanced
Initiative; 3=more or less equal
Conflict1 1=not at all; 2=once or twice; 3=several times More contact if no conflicts
Geographical distance3 Interval Less contact if distance increases
Concern given and received1,2 1=given and received not at all (dependent variable)
2=given once or twice; received not at all
3=given several times; received not at all
4=given not at all; received once or twice
5=given and received once or twice
6=given several times; received once or twice
7=given not at all; received several times
8=given once or twice; received several times
9=given and received several times
Face-to-face contact3 1=never; 2=once; 3=several times (dependent variable)
4=at least monthly; 5=at least weekly; 6=several times a week or daily

Superscripts 1–3 specify in which analysis the variable is used. Geographical distance is used only in the second model of Analysis 3.

It is important to note that nearly 7% did not respond on the questions concerning relationship with sibling A (financial balance, financial help given, financial help received, initiative contact, conflict, concern given, and received). The questions for these variables were not asked if the respondent did not have any face-to-face contact with his or her sibling A over the past 12 months. Therefore, I will include a second analysis where I have added a category ‘missing’ (Analysis 2). This category indicates that the respondent has not been in contact with his/her sibling over the past 12 months. In order to confirm the findings of Analysis 2, I use a measure for face-to-face contact (n=7406; Analysis 3). A first model does not control for geographical distance, as for more than 10% of the respondents these data are lacking. A second model including a Euclidean distance measure allows estimating the effect for relatedness while controlling for the effect of distance (see Dykstra et al., 2004).

For the analyses, I will thus use ‘concern given and concern received’ (Analyses 1–2) and then face-to-face contact as a dependent measure (Analysis 3). Both measures reflect social investment. Multinomial logistic regression (MLR) was used to investigate the independent effect of genetic relatedness on the dependent variable (Hosmer & Lemeshow, 1989, Menard, 1995, Pampel, 2000). Multinomial logistic regression as statistical technique is relatively free of assumptions and statistically robust. Unlike ordinary least square regression, parameters are estimated by maximum likelihood. As a parameter selection procedure, forward stepwise was used. Model outcomes were only marginally different in terms of model fit and Nagelkerke (1991)R2 when backward stepwise was used instead. Here I will report the likelihood ratio (LLR) tests for variables in the model and parameter estimates (see Peng, Lee, & Ingersoll, 2002). As many independent variables are used, I will not discuss all effects. I focus on the independent effect of genetic relatedness. For each MLR, the parameter estimates for PHS or MHS are provided if significantly different from FS. I present Wald statistics and p values for the LLR tests. The Wald statistic allows examining the significance of single parameters (Pampel, 2000). The p value for LLR is based on a χ2 test statistic: the difference between the reduced, that is, without a certain variable, and the final model in terms of −2 log likelihood (−2LL) (Pampel, 2000). The χ2 test is set against the null hypothesis that all parameter estimates corresponding to a variable equal zero. These values are therefore the same for different parameter estimates from the same MLR.

3. Results

3.1. Assumption

Respondents who were not living with both natural parents at birth were significantly more likely to live in a family structure with their biological mother, that is, mother only or mother and stepfather, than in a family structure with their biological father (binomial test: p=2×10−15). For all family transitions until 16 years old, respondents were more likely to live with a family containing their biological mother than in a family with their biological father (all binomial tests: p<.005).2

3.2. Analysis 1: concern given and received over the past 3 months

Fig. 1 shows that in more than 35% of the PHS dyads, siblings did not show any concern for each other. For MHS dyads, nearly one quarter was unconcerned for each other, while nearly 15% of the dyads where siblings are fully related showed this lack of concern for each other.


View full-size image.

Fig. 1. Sibling type and concern given and received over the past 3 months (χ2=55.4, df=16, p=3.05×10−6).


The variables listed with superscript ‘1’ in Table 1 were used for the model. Education difference did not independently influence the likelihood of giving and receiving concern in the MLR. Sibling type proved to be a significant independent predictor of concern given and received (−2LL=23447.56, χ2=27.17, df=16, p=.04). The model consisting of these predictors has a Nagelkerke R2 of .207, which is good according to standards in the social sciences (model fit: −2LL=23420.39, χ2=1525.67, df=176, p<.0001).

Parameter estimates for not having given any concern and not having received any concern vs. having given and received concern are presented in Table 2. The Wald statistic allows examining the significance of single parameters (Pampel, 2000). Parameter estimates are best interpreted in terms of odds ratios or Exp(λ), which allows examining effect size. The odds are the probability of category membership Xi divided by the probability of membership of the reference category. For instance, the odds, that is, the probability of not having given and received any concern for their sibling vs. the probability of having given and received concern several times, for respondents who have a brother as sibling A are 2.22 times3 the odds for respondents who have a sister. This effect is found while controlling for the other variables in the model. If this is phrased differently: respondents are 2.22 times more likely to not have given and received any concern instead of several times over the past 3 months if they have a brother instead of a sister. For the discussion of the results I use this shorthand version, yet it is important to bear in mind that ‘times more likely not to have given and received any concern’ refers to a comparison between categories in terms of odds. Thus, respondents are significantly more likely to have given and received concern several times over the past 3 months by their sibling vs. not at all, if their sibling was a sister instead of a brother. An example of an odds ratio for an interval variable, an ‘increase of one sibling,’ has an odds ratio of 1.12. For each sibling the respondent has apart from sibling A, it becomes 1.12 times more likely that he or she has not given and received any concern, instead of having given and received concern several times over the past 3 months.

Table 2.

Parameter estimates for concern given and received none (vs. given and received several times)

Concern given None λ S.E. Wald df pWald Exp (λ) pLLR
Concern received None
Intercept −7.67 1.62 22.34 1 <.001
Sibling type PHS 1.63 0.41 15.50 1 <.001 5.12 .04
MHS 0.60 0.34 3.19 1 .074 1.83
FS 0 0
Education Incomplete/primary 1.44 0.21 46.44 1 <.001 4.22 <.001
Lower vocational 1.55 0.18 71.14 1 <.001 4.72
Lower general secondary 1.19 0.19 37.40 1 <.001 3.28
Medium general secondary 0.65 0.25 6.83 1 .009 1.92
Upper general secondary 0.44 0.27 2.81 1 .094 1.56
Intermediate vocational 0.89 0.17 26.17 1 <.001 2.43
Higher vocational 0.46 0.17 6.96 1 .008 1.58
University or postgraduate 0 0
Financial balance Respondent gives more 1.12 0.31 12.94 1 <.001 1.95 <.001
Other gives more 0.45 0.29 2.34 1 .126 1.57
More or less equal 0 0
Initiative contact Usually my initiative 0.66 0.20 11.27 1 <.001 2.29 <.001
Usually other's initiative −0.17 0.18 0.89 1 .346 0.84
More or less equal 0 0
Conflict Not at all −1.03 0.28 13.61 1 <.001 0.36 <.001
Once or twice −1.32 0.32 17.24 1 <.001 0.27
Several times 0 0
Age difference (change by one year) 0.03 0.01 7.47 1 .006 1.03 .014
Gender of sibling A Female −0.80 0.08 90.65 1 <.001 0.45 <.001
Number of siblings (change by one sibling) 0.12 0.02 43.44 1 <.001 1.12 <.001
Gender respondent Female −1.08 0.09 159.21 1 <.001 0.34 <.001
Age (change by one year) 0.01 0.00 19.25 1 <.001 1.01 <.001
Financial help received No 1.57 0.61 6.64 1 .010 4.80 <.001
Financial help given No 1.96 0.53 13.67 1 <.001 7.12 <.001

Parameter estimates are set to 0 for reference categories.

Paternal half-siblings are 5.12 times more likely not to have shown any concern for their sibling, instead of several times, over the past 3 months than FS. MHS are 1.83 times more likely not to have shown any concern for their sibling, instead of several times, than FS. These effects for PHS and MHS are found while controlling for the other variables in the model. Yet, the difference between MHS and FS is not significant at the 5% level [95% confidence interval for Exp(λ), 0.94–3.55].

Respondents are 3.46 times more likely to state they have shown concern once or twice while their sibling showed no concern for them, instead of both siblings having shown concern several times over the past 3 months, if they are PHS than if they are FS (pWald=.018; table not shown). Respondents are 5.97 times more likely to state they have shown concern several times while their sibling showed no concern for them, instead of both siblings having shown concern several times, if they are PHS than if they are FS (pWald=.012; table not shown).

3.3. Analysis 2: concern given and received over the past 3 months (including missing)

For nearly 40% of the PHS, there were no data available on concern in each other, indicating that these persons did not have contact over the past 12 months (Fig. 2). For MHS there were no data available for 30% of the dyads. Only 5% of the FS did not have any data on concern given and received. This indicates that PHS and MHS are more likely not to have had any contact with their sibling over the past 12 months.


View full-size image.

Fig. 2. Sibling type and concern given and received (including missing) over the past 3 months (χ2=144.28, df=18, p=9.54×10−22).


All variables indicated with superscript ‘2’ in Table 1 proved significant predictors for concern given and received. Sibling type proved to be a highly significant independent predictor of concern given and received (−2LL=27314.87, χ2=100.81, df=18. p=1.57×10−13). This model has a Nagelkerke R2 of .154 (model fit: −2LL=27214.06, χ2=1198.83, df=135, p<.0001) (Table 3).

Table 3.

Parameter estimates for concern given several times and received missing (vs. given and received several times)

Concern given missing λ S.E. Wald df pWald Exp (λ) pLLR
Concern received missing
Intercept −3.83 0.35 119.68 1 <.001
Relatedness PHS 2.65 0.39 46.56 1 <.001 14.13 <.001
MHS 1.82 0.31 34.66 1 <.001 6.16
FS 0 0
Education Incomplete/primary 1.70 0.30 33.08 1 <.001 5.47 <.001
Lower vocational 1.64 0.28 34.98 1 <.001 5.16
Lower general secondary 1.25 0.29 18.62 1 <.001 3.48
Medium general secondary 0.97 0.36 7.38 1 .007 2.65
Upper general secondary 0.45 0.38 1.37 1 .243 1.56
Intermediate vocational 1.04 0.27 15.08 1 <.001 2.84
Higher vocational 0.65 0.27 5.79 1 .016 1.92
University or postgraduate 0 0
Education difference (increase by one category) 0.07 0.03 4.55 1 .033 1.07 .001
Age difference (increase by one year) 0.05 0.01 14.44 1 <.001 1.05 .009
Gender of sibling A Female −0.51 0.11 21.30 1 <.001 0.60 <.001
Number of siblings (increase by one sibling) 0.07 0.02 10.84 1 .001 1.08 <.001
Gender respondent Female −0.79 0.11 47.57 1 <.001 0.46 <.001
Age (increase by one year) 0.03 0.00 69.90 1 <.001 1.03 <.001

Parameter estimates are set to 0 for reference categories.

Data are 14.13 times more likely to be missing, instead of indicating ‘giving and received several times’ for PHS than for FS. For MHS, data are 6.16 times more likely to be missing than indicating that concern was given and received several times over the past 3 months than for FS. This indicates PHS and MHS are more likely not to have been in contact over the past 12 months with their sibling than FS. Respondents are 4.81 to have shown and received no concern if they are PHS than if they are FS (pWald<.001; table not shown). If respondents are MHS they are 1.90 times more likely not to have shown or received any concern than if they are FS (pWald=.049; table not shown). The odds ratio for concern given once or twice but received none for PHS is 3.32 (pWald=.017; table not shown). For concern given several times but received none the odds ratio for PHS is 5.16 (pWald=.016; table not shown).

3.4. Analysis 3: face-to-face contact over the past 12 months

In line with Fig. 2, over 40% of the PHS did not have any face-to-face contact with their sibling. Nearly 40% of the MHS did not have any face-to-face contact with their sibling, while only 10% of the FS did not have any contact. Only fully related siblings indicated they had face-to-face contact on a daily basis (Fig. 3). Therefore, this category was merged with ‘several times a week’ for the MLR.


View full-size image.

Fig. 3. Sibling type and face-to-face contact over the past 12 months (Somers' Dyx=0.483, p=1.19×10−28).


All variables indicated with superscript ‘3’ in Table 1 proved significant predictors for face-to-face contact. The model has a Nagelkerke R2 of .142 (model fit: −2LL=20843.99, χ2=1043.46, df=75, p<.0001) (Table 4).

Table 4.

Parameter estimates for face-to-face contact not at all (vs. daily or several times a week)

Face-to-face contact Not at all λ S.E. Wald df pWald Exp (λ) pLLR
Intercept −1.84 0.40 21.76 1 <.001
Sibling type PHS 3.41 1.03 10.90 1 .001 30.22 <.001
MHS 1.81 0.49 13.52 1 <.001 6.11
FS 0 0
Education Incomplete/primary −0.55 0.33 2.76 1 .097 0.58 <.001
Lower vocational −0.83 0.30 7.54 1 .006 0.43
Lower general secondary −0.54 0.31 2.96 1 .085 0.58
Medium general secondary −0.28 0.38 0.54 1 .462 0.76
Upper general secondary −0.49 0.41 1.43 1 .232 0.61
Intermediate vocational −0.66 0.29 5.28 1 .022 0.52
Higher vocational −0.08 0.30 0.08 1 .779 0.92
University or postgraduate 0 0
Education difference (increase by one category) 0.09 0.04 5.55 1 .018 1.09 .017
Age difference (change by one year) 0.03 0.02 3.51 1 .061 1.03 .015
Gender of sibling A Female −0.44 0.13 11.36 1 .001 0.65 <.001
Number of siblings (change by one sibling) 0.05 0.03 2.88 1 .090 1.05 .007
Gender respondent Female −0.47 0.13 12.07 1 .001 0.63 .002
Age (increase by one year) 0.07 0.01 160.76 1 <.001 1.07 <.001

Parameter estimates are set to 0 for reference categories.

PHS are 30.22 times more likely than FS not to have had any contact instead of having contact daily or several times a week. MHS are 6.22 times more likely than FS not to have had any contact instead of having contact daily or several times a week.

PHS are also 20.5 times more likely than FS to have had contact once and 7.99 times more likely than FS to have had contact several times instead of daily or several times a week (pWald=.004 and .043, respectively; tables not shown).

Controlling for geographical distance improves the model fit (model fit: −2LL=16714, χ2=2194.92, df=75, p<.0001, Nagelkerke R2=.306). After controlling for distance, age difference is not a significant predictor (at α=.05). In this model, the odds ratios for having face-to-face contact not at all, instead of a few times a week or daily, are 22.48 for PHS and 8.84 for MHS vs. FS (pWald=.003 and <.001, respectively; tables not shown). The odds ratio for having contact ‘once’ instead of a few times a week or daily for PHS vs. FS is 17.18 (pWald=.009; table not shown). Unlike for the first model, for MHS the odds ratio for once, instead of a few times a week or daily, is significant (pWald=.015, odds ratio=4.93; table not shown). For PHS the odds ratio for ‘several times’, instead of daily or several times a week, is not significant anymore (pWald=.071; table not shown).

4. Discussion

The results clearly show that the type of sibling dyad independently and quite strongly influences the social dynamics of the sibling dyad. Social factors also heavily determine the mutual social investments in the dyads. This is evident from the difference in Nagelkerke R2 between Analysis 1 and Analysis 2. In line with social exchange theory, balanced relationships were stronger and financial investments mirrored social investments. Also, the more similar siblings are, the stronger their relationship. In line with previous findings on women as kin keepers, women are significantly more likely to maintain stronger relationships with other kin (Fischer, 1982, Lee et al., 1990, Rosenthal, 1985, White & Riedmann, 1992). As found in previous research (Lee et al., 1990), there is also some support for the finding that sister–sister dyads are stronger than sister–brother or brother–brother dyads. Contrary to the argument that kinship ties become less important with rising education (Fischer, 1982, Treas & Bengston, 1987; see Beck & Beck-Gernsheim, 2002), in this sample, higher educational attainment appears to lead to more social investment by siblings.

However, while controlling for these other factors affecting the sibling dyad (Table 1), there is an independent effect for sibling type. Consistent with our hypothesis, not only PHS but also MHS, who are likely to have been raised together, displayed lower rates of social investment than FS. This finding suggests that a psychological mechanism besides childhood propinquity, such as third-party knowledge, affects investment in siblings. Yet, for all analyses, the odds ratios for MHS vs. FS dyads were much weaker than for PHS vs. FS dyads, underlining the importance of cohabitation during childhood for sibling relationships (see Lieberman et al., 2003). Interestingly, PHS are significantly more likely than FS to claim there is an imbalance in their relationship, that is, that they show concern without receiving any concern. Yet, no such effect is found for MHS. This can be interpreted as PHS reporting a worse relationship than MHS and FS.

One of the drawbacks of this study is that self-reported, rather crude, measures of investment and survey measures for genetic relatedness were used. Moreover, it is unclear whether or not the observed differences in sibling relations have any measurable effect on inclusive fitness in modern societies. This study did not include any data on relations between step-siblings. Although previous research has shown that sibling dyads within stepfamilies display more conflict (Hetherington, 1988), it is unclear whether these sibling relations are significantly different from half-sibling dyads.

Consistent with previous findings (Hames, 1979, Hill & Dunbar, 2003) and with Emlen's (1997) prediction, I conclude that siblings interact significantly less if they are not fully related, all else being equal. Given that there are significant differences between MHS and FS, it appears that another psychological mechanism besides childhood propinquity regulates investment in siblings. Co-residence during childhood does play an important role, however, with PHS showing less interaction than MHS. These findings appear robust and cannot be attributed to other social factors (Table 1). Contrary to the individualization perspective on the family (Beck & Beck-Gernsheim, 2002, Beck-Gernsheim, 1998), genetic relatedness still influences relationship dynamics between siblings.

5. Conclusion

Contrary to the assumptions of individualization theory of the family, genetic relatedness (still) strongly influences the frequency of interaction between siblings. Sibling dyads where the siblings are not fully related displayed lower rates of social investment. It appears that besides childhood co-residence another psychological mechanism regulates investment. Yet, cohabitation during childhood is an important determinant of the social dynamics between siblings in adult life, given that PHS displayed much lower social investment than MHS. This effect of genetic relatedness, mediated by childhood cohabitation, on social investment appears robust and cannot be attributed to other social factors influencing the sibling dyad.

Acknowledgments

The NKPS is funded by grant 480-10-009 from the Major Investments Fund of the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research, and by the Netherlands Interdisciplinary Demographic Institute (NIDI), Utrecht University, the University of Amsterdam, and Tilburg University. The author thanks Daniel Nettle, the Evolutionary Psychology Research Group at the University of Liverpool, the Evolution and Behaviour Study Group (Belgium), and Mark Nelissen for their valuable comments; Toon Kuppens and Susanne Shultz for statistical advice; and the Lazard foundation for funding. The author also thanks NIDI for access to the data and the users of the NKPS e-forum, especially R. Van Galen, for answering questions concerning the data set. The reviewers and Daniel Fessler provided comments that substantially improved an earlier draft.

References

Alvard, 2003 1.Alvard M. Kinship, lineage and an evolutionary perspective on cooperative hunting in Indonesia. Human Nature. 2003;14:129–163.

Aoki, 2005 2.Aoki K. Avoidance and prohibition of brother–sister sex in humans. Population Ecology. 2005;47:13–19.

Aquilino, 1991 3.Aquilino WS. Family structure and home-leaving: A further specification of the relationship. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1991;53:999–1010.

Beck, 1993 4.Beck U. The risk society. London: Sage; 1993 [1992];.

Beck & Beck-Gernsheim, 2002 5.Beck U, Beck-Gernsheim E. Individualization: Institutionalized individualism and its social and political consequences. London: Sage; 2002;.

Beck-Gernsheim, 1998 6.Beck-Gernsheim E. On the way to a post-familial family: From a community of need to elective affinities. Theory, Culture & Society. 1998;15:53–70.

Berté, 1988 7.Berté NA. K'ekchi horticultural labor exchange: Productive and reproductive implications. In:  Betzig LL,  Bogerhoff Mulder M,  Turke PW editor. Human reproductive behaviour: A darwinian perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University press; 1988;p. 83–96.

Betzig & Turke, 1986 8.Betzig L, Turke PW. Food sharing on Ifaluk. Current Anthropology. 1986;27:397–400. CrossRef

Bevc & Silverman, 1993 9.Bevc I, Silverman I. Early proximity and intimacy between siblings and incestuous behavior: A test of the Westermarck theory. Ethology and Sociobiology. 1993;14:171–181.

Bevc & Silverman, 2000 10.Bevc I, Silverman I. Early separation and sibling incest: A test of the revised Westermarck theory. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2000;21:151–161.

Blau, 1964 11.Blau PM. Exchange and power in social life. New York: Wiley; 1964;.

Bowles & Posel, 2005 12.Bowles S, Posel D. Genetic relatedness predicts migrants' workers remittances to their families. Nature. 2005;434:380–383. CrossRef

Burnstein et al., 1994 13.Burnstein E, Crandall C, Kitayama S. Some Neo-Darwinian decision rules for altruism: Weighing cues for inclusive fitness as a function of the biological importance of the decision. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;67:733–789.

Cashdan, 1985 14.Cashdan E. Coping with risk: Reciprocity among the Basarwa of Northern Botswana. Man. 1985;20:454–474.

Chagnon & Bugos, 1979 15.Chagnon N, Bugos P. Kinship and social conflict: An analysis of a Yanomamö ax fight. In:  Chagnon N,  Irons W editor. Evolutionary biology and human behavior. Massachusetts: Duxbury Press; 1979;p. 213–238.

Cherlin & Furstenberg, 1994 16.Cherlin AJ, Furstenberg FF. Stepfamilies in the United States: A reconsideration. Annual Review of Sociology. 1994;20:359–381.

Daly & Wilson, 1981 17.Daly M, Wilson MI. Abuse and neglect of children in evolutionary perspective. In:  Alexander RD,  Tinkle DW editor. Natural selection and social behavior: Recent research and new theory. Oxford: Blackwell; 1981;p. 405–417.

Daly & Wilson, 1982 18.Daly M, Wilson MI. Homicide and kinship. American Anthropologist. 1982;84:372–378.

Daly & Wilson, 1985 19.Daly M, Wilson MI. Child abuse and other risks of not living with both parents. Ethology and Sociobiology. 1985;6:197–210.

Daly & Wilson, 1988 20.Daly M, Wilson MI. Evolutionary social psychology and homicide. Science. 1988;242:519–524. MEDLINE

DeBruine, 2002 21.DeBruine LM. Facial resemblance enhances trust. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 2002;269:1307–1312.

DeBruine, 2004a 22.DeBruine LM. Facial resemblance increases the attractiveness of same-sex faces more than other-sex faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 2004;271:2085–2090.

DeBruine, 2004b 23.DeBruine LM. Resemblance to self increases the appeal of child faces to both men and women. Evolution and Human Behaviour. 2004;25:142–154.

DeBruine, 2005 24.DeBruine LM. Trustworthy but not lust-worthy: Context-specific effects of facial resemblance. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 2005;272:919–922.

De Graaf, 1997 25.De Graaf A. Kinderen van gescheiden ouders (‘Children of divorced parents’). Demos. 1997;http://www.nidi.knaw.nl/web/html/public/demos/dm97021.html(accessed online on 19–4–2006).

Dunbar et al., 1995 26.Dunbar RIM, Clark A, Hurst NL. Conflict and cooperation amongst Vikings: Contingent behavioural decisions. Ethology and Sociobiology. 1995;16:233–246.

Dunbar & Spoors, 1995 27.Dunbar RIM, Spoors M. Social networks, support cliques and kinship. Human Nature. 1995;6:273–290.

Dunn & Plomin, 1991 28.Dunn J, Plomin R. Why are siblings so different? The significance of differences in sibling experiences within the family. Family Process. 1991;30:271–283. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Dykstra et al., 2004 29.Dykstra PA, Kalmijn M, Knijn TCM, Komter AE, Liefbroer AC, Mulder CH. Codebook of the Netherlands Kinship Panel Study, a multi-actor, multi-method panel study on solidarity in family relationships, Wave 1. NKPS Working Paper No. 1. The Hague: Netherlands Interdisciplinary Demographic Institute; 2004;(version of July 2005).

Emlen, 1995 30.Emlen ST. An evolutionary theory of the family. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America. 1995;92:8092–8099.

Emlen, 1997 31.Emlen ST. The evolutionary study of human family systems. Social Science Information. 1997;34:563–589.

Fessler & Navarrete, 2004 32.Fessler DMT, Navarrete CD. Third-party attitudes toward sibling incest evidence for Westermarck's hypotheses. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2004;25:277–294.

Fischer, 1982 33.Fischer C. To dwell among friends: Personal networks in town and city. Chicago: University of Chicago press; 1982;.

Giddens, 1991 34.Giddens A. Modernity and self identity. London: Polity; 1991;.

Gurven, 2004 35.Gurven M. To give or to give not: The behavioural ecology of food transfers. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2004;27:543–583.

Gurven et al., 2000 36.Gurven M, Hill K, Kaplan H, Hurtado A, Lyles R. Food transfers among Hiwi foragers of Venezuela: Tests of reciprocity. Human Ecology. 2000;28:171–218.

Hames, 1979 37.Hames RD. Relatedness and interaction among the Ye'kwana: A preliminary analysis. In:  Chagnon N,  Irons W editor. Evolutionary biology and human behavior. Massachusetts: Duxbury Press; 1979;p. 238–249.

Hames, 1987 38.Hames RD. Relatedness and garden labor exchange among the Ye'kwana. Ethology and Sociobiology. 1987;8:259–284.

Hamilton, 1964 39.Hamilton WD. The genetical evolution of social behaviour I, II. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 1964;7:1–52. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Hetherington, 1988 40.Hetherington EM. Parents, children and siblings six years after divorce. In:  Hinde RA,  Hinde JS editor. Relationships within the family: Mutual influences. Oxford: Clarendon Press; 1988;p. 311–331.

Hill & Dunbar, 2003 41.Hill RA, Dunbar RIM. Social network size in humans. Human Nature. 2003;14:53–72.

Homans, 1951 42.Homans GC. The human group. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul; 1951;.

Homans, 1958 43.Homans GC. Social behavior as exchange. American Journal of Sociology. 1958;63:597–606. CrossRef

Hosmer & Lemeshow, 1989 44.Hosmer D, Lemeshow S. Applied logistic regression. New York: Wiley & Sons; 1989;.

Jankowiak & Diderich, 2000 45.Jankowiak W, Diderich M. Sibling solidarity in a polygamous community in the USA: Unpacking inclusive fitness. Evolution and Human Behaviour. 2000;21:125–139.

Johnson & Johnson, 1991 46.Johnson SB, Johnson RC. Support and conflict of kinsmen in Norse Earldoms, Icelandic families and English royalty. Ethology and Sociobiology. 1991;12:211–220.

Kana'Iaupuni et al., 2005 47.Kana'Iaupuni SM, Donato KM, Thompson-Colon T, Stainback M. Counting on kin: Social networks, social support, and child health status. Social Forces. 2005;83:1137–1164.

Korchmaros & Kenny, 2001 48.Korchmaros JD, Kenny DA. Emotional closeness as a mediator for the effect of genetic relatedness on altruism. Psychological Science. 2001;12:262–265. MEDLINE

Kruger, 2001 49.Kruger DJ. Psychological aspects of adaptations for kin directed altruistic helping behaviours. Social Behavior and Personality. 2001;29:323–330.

Lee et al., 1990 50.Lee TR, Mancini JA, Maxwell JA. Sibling relationships in adulthood: Contact patterns and motivations. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1990;52:431–440.

Lieberman et al., 2003 51.Lieberman D, Tooby J, Cosmides L. Does morality have a biological basis? An empirical test of the factors governing moral sentiments relating to incest. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 2003;270:819–826.

Madsen et al., in press 52.Madsen, E. A., Tunney, R. J., Fieldman, G., Plotkin, H. C., Dunbar, R. I. M., Richardson, & J. M. et al. (in press). Kinship and altruism: A cross-cultural experimental study. British Journal of Psychology.

McPherson et al., 2001 53.McPherson M, Smith-Lovin J, Cook JM. Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology. 2001;27:425–444.

Menard, 1995 54.Menard S. Applied logistic analysis, quantitative applications in the social sciences series #106. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 1995;.

Miller & Darlington, 2002 55.Miller RJ, Darlington Y. Who supports? The providers of social support to dual-parent families caring for young children. Journal of Community Psychology. 2002;30:461–475. CrossRef

Molm, 1997 56.Molm LD. Coercive power in social exchange. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1997;.

Morgan, 1979 57.Morgan C. Eskimo hunting groups, social kinship and the possibility of kin selection in humans. Ethology and Sociobiology. 1979;1:83–91.

Nagelkerke, 1991 58.Nagelkerke NJD. A note on a general definition of the coefficient of determination. Biometrika. 1991;78:691–692. CrossRef

Neyer & Lang, 2003 59.Neyer FJ, Lang FR. Blood is thicker than water: Kinship orientation across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;82:310–321.

Oates & Wilson, 2002 60.Oates K, Wilson MI. Nominal kinship cues facilitate altruism. Proceedings of the Royal Society London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 2002;269:105–109.

Olson, 1965 61.Olson M. The logic of collective action: Public goods and the theory of groups. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 1965;.

Pampel, 2000 62.Pampel FC. Logistic regression: A primer, quantitative applications in the social sciences series #132. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 2000;.

Park & Schaller, 2005 63.Park JH, Schaller M. Does attitude similarity serve as a heuristic cue for kinship? Evidence of an implicit cognitive association. Evolution and Human Behaviour. 2005;26:158–170.

Peng et al., 2002 64.Peng C-YJ, Lee KL, Ingersoll G. An introduction to logistic regression analysis and reporting. Journal of Educational Research. 2002;96:1–14.

Plomin & Daniels, 1987 65.Plomin R, Daniels D. Why are children in the same family so different from one another. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1987;10:1–60.

Rosenthal, 1985 66.Rosenthal CJ. Kinkeeping in the familial division of labour. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1985;47:965–974.

Stets & Burke, 2000 67.Stets JE, Burke PJ. Identity theory and social identity theory. Social Psychology Quarterly. 2000;63:224–237. CrossRef

Stewart, 2005 68.Stewart SD. How the birth of a child affects involvement with step-children. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 2005;67:461–473.

Sulloway, 1996 69.Sulloway F. Born to rebel. New York: Pantheon; 1996;.

Sulloway, 2001 70.Sulloway F. Birth order, sibling competition, and human behaviour. In:  Davies PS,  Holcomb HR editor. Conceptual challenges in evolutionary psychology: Innovative research strategies. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers; 2001;p. 39–83.

Treas & Bengston, 1987 71.Treas J, Bengston VL. The family in later years. In:  Sussman MB,  Steinmetz SK editor. Handbook of marriage and the family. New York: Plenum; 1987;p. 625–648.

Wedekind & Furi, 1997 72.Wedekind C, Furi S. Body odor preferences in men and women: Do they aim for specific MHC combinations or simply heterozygosity?. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. 1997;264:1471–1479. MEDLINE | CrossRef

White & Riedmann, 1992 73.White LK, Riedmann A. Ties among adult siblings. Social Forces. 1992;71:85–102.

Zvoch, 1999 74.Zvoch K. Family type and investment in education: A comparison of genetic and stepparent families. Evolution and Human Behaviour. 1999;20:453–464.

Evolution and Behaviour Research Group, Division of Psychology, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom

Henry Wellcome Building for Neuroecology (Newcastle University), Framlington Place, NE2 4HH, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom.

1 Due to the setup of the data set, sibling A could not be a step-sibling of the respondent: sibling A was a full, half-, or adopted sibling.

2 I did not analyze family transitions containing less than 0.5% of the total sample (n=37).

3 2.22=1/0.45; reference categories can be ‘switched’ by inverting parameter estimates.

PII: S1090-5138(06)00094-8

doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.10.001



2007:12:16