Поиск по сайту




Пишите нам: info@ethology.ru

Follow etholog on Twitter

Система Orphus

Новости
Библиотека
Видео
Разное
Кросс-культурный метод
Старые форумы
Рекомендуем
Не в тему

список статей


Visual adaptation to masculine and feminine faces influences generalized preferences and perceptions of trustworthiness

Gavin Buckinghama, Lisa M. DeBruineb, Anthony C. Littlec, Lisa L.M. Wellinga, Claire A. Conwaya, Bernard P. Tiddemand, Benedict C. Jonesa

1. Introduction

2. Methods

2.1. Stimuli

2.2. Participants

2.3. Procedure

2.4. Initial processing of data

3. Results

4. Discussion

Acknowledgment

References

Copyright

1. Introduction

Face preferences and perceptions influence important social interactions including partner and associate choices, hiring decisions, and voting behavior (e.g., Langlois et al., 2000, Todorov et al., 2005). Furthermore, many researchers have highlighted the importance of identifying the factors that influence the development of these attitudes (DeBruine, 2005, Little et al., 2005, Perrett et al., 2002, Rhodes et al., 2003). Although factors such as changes in levels of sex hormones influence attitudes to faces (e.g., DeBruine et al., 2005, Jones et al., 2005, Jones et al., 2005, Penton-Voak et al., 1999), visual experience also plays an important role in shaping how we perceive others (Little et al., 2005, Perrett et al., 2002, Rhodes et al., 2003, Webster et al., 2004).

Adaptation to faces following visual experience (i.e., weakened responses of neurons that code a given type of stimulus following habituation to that type of stimulus; Loffler et al., 2005, Winston et al., 2004) influences perceptions of faces (Leopold et al., 2001, Leopold et al., 2005, Little et al., 2005, Rhodes et al., 2003, Rhodes et al., 2004, Webster et al., 2004). Adaptation to faces varying in race, sex, sexual dimorphism of shape, identity, eye spacing, and expanded or compressed features has been shown to influence subsequent perceptions of the normality (Leopold et al., 2001, Leopold et al., 2005, Little et al., 2005, Rhodes et al., 2003, Rhodes et al., 2004, Webster et al., 2004) and attractiveness (Little et al., 2005, Rhodes et al., 2003) of faces such that faces similar to those seen previously are judged more normal and attractive than would otherwise be the case. Many researchers have emphasized the possible direct and indirect benefits of imprinting-like effects on mate preferences in both humans and nonhumans (as opposed to relatively inflexible innate preferences; for reviews, see Little, Burt, Penton-Voak, & Perrett, 2001 and Perrett et al., 2002), and visual adaptation has been identified as a plausible proximate mechanism for imprinting-like effects on face preferences (Little et al., 2005, Perrett et al., 2002). “Face aftereffects” (Leopold et al., 2001, Leopold et al., 2005, Little et al., 2005, Rhodes et al., 2003, Rhodes et al., 2004, Webster et al., 2004) reflect changes in the responses of neurons that code faces (Loffler et al., 2005, Winston et al., 2004) and cannot be explained by retinal adaptation because they are robust to changes in the size and retinal location of the stimuli between adaptation and test phases (Leopold et al., 2001, Leopold et al., 2005). Furthermore, bigger face aftereffects occur when the orientation or sex of the faces shown in the adaptation and test phases are congruent than when they are incongruent, suggesting that different neural populations code different subcategories of faces (Little et al., 2005, Rhodes et al., 2004).

Although previous experiments have established that adaptation to faces increases subsequent attraction to novel similar faces (Little et al., 2005, Rhodes et al., 2003), it is unclear if face aftereffects also influence attributions of other prosocial traits that are important for social interactions (e.g., trustworthiness; see DeBruine, 2005 and Perrett et al., 1998). Although the “beauty is good” stereotype is a prevalent way to explain how humans attribute personality characteristics such as trustworthiness to faces (e.g., Langlois et al., 2000), judgments of faces in the contexts of mate choice and general social interaction may have different functional explanations and may be affected in different ways by different aspects of facial appearance (e.g., age and cues of kinship). For example, facial self-resemblance increases perceptions of the trustworthiness of opposite-sex faces but decreases perceptions of these same faces' sexual attractiveness and does not influence attractiveness judgments made in the context of long-term relationships (which arguably reflect a mixture of sexual attraction and prosocial regard, DeBruine, 2005; see Little et al., 2001 and Little, Jones, Penton-Voak, Burt, & Perrett, 2002 for further discussion of the distinction between face preferences expressed in the contexts of short- and long-term relationships). For this reason, it is not obvious that factors affecting the attractiveness of faces will affect their perceived trustworthiness in the same way.

To investigate the extent to which adaptation to faces has similar effects on attraction to novel similar faces and perceptions of their trustworthiness, here we compared the effect of adaptation to faces varying in sexual dimorphism of 2D shape on attraction with sexual dimorphism in male faces and attributions of the trustworthiness of masculine and feminine male faces. Male faces were used because attraction to sexual dimorphism in male faces is known to be variable (see Fink and Penton-Voak, 2002 and Penton-Voak and Perrett, 2000 for reviews), and therefore, ceiling effects on preferences are less likely to mask face aftereffects than if female faces were used, and because sex-contingent face aftereffects have previously been reported (Little et al., 2005). Sexual dimorphism of 2D shape was manipulated using computer graphic methods (prototype-based transformations; Rowland & Perrett, 1995, Tiddeman et al., 2001) that have been previously used to investigate attraction (Jones et al., 2005, Little et al., 2001, Little et al., 2002, Penton-Voak et al., 1999) and attributions of personality characteristics (Perrett et al., 1998) to masculine and feminine faces as well as to manufacture stimuli for studies of face aftereffects (Little et al., 2005). The adaptation paradigm we used was a slightly modified version of that used by Little et al. (2005) to demonstrate that adaptation to faces varying in sexual dimorphism of 2D shape influences attraction to masculine and feminine faces.

2. Methods

2.1. Stimuli

Male and female prototype face images were manufactured by averaging the shape and color information from 20 young (17–23 years old) adult male faces (to create a male prototype face image) and 20 young (18–23 years old) adult female faces (to create a female prototype face image) using methods reported in Perrett et al. (1998) and elsewhere (Rowland & Perrett, 1995, Tiddeman et al., 2001). Following Little et al. (2005), the 20 male face images were then transformed by +50% of the differences in 2D shape between the male and female prototypes to create masculinized versions and by −50% of the differences in shape to create feminized versions (see Rowland and Perrett, 1995 and Tiddeman et al., 2001 for technical details of this method and Jones et al., 2005, Little et al., 2001, Little et al., 2002, 2004, and Penton-Voak et al., 1999 for other studies that have used this method to manipulate sexual dimorphism in faces). This procedure generates masculinized and feminized versions of the male faces that differ equally from average, differ only in 2D shape and are matched in terms of identity (see Little et al., 2005 and Perrett et al., 1998 for discussions of these issues). An example of a masculinized and feminized face used in the experiment is shown in Fig. 1. It is noteworthy that while other methods for manipulating masculinity of faces have been used in some other studies (e.g., manipulating facial appearance using composites of faces perceived to be high or low in terms of masculinity; Johnston et al., 2001), these methods produce effects on face perceptions that are equivalent to those produced using the methods used in our current study (DeBruine et al., 2006).


View full-size image.

Fig. 1. Examples of masculinized (left) and feminized (right) versions of a male face image used in the experiment. Note that the faces differ in sexual dimorphism of 2D shape but are matched in other regards (e.g., identity).


2.2. Participants

Fifty-five participants (mean age=22.46 years, S.D.=3.22 years, 34 females) took part in the experiment.

2.3. Procedure

To assess attraction or attributions of trust to masculinity in male faces prior to the adaptation phase, we showed participants five pairs of male face images where each pair consisted of a masculinized and feminized version of the same identity. Twenty-six of the participants were asked to choose the face in each pair that they considered more attractive and to indicate the strength of this preference by choosing from the options “much more attractive,” “more attractive,” “somewhat more attractive,” and “slightly more attractive.” The remaining 29 participants were shown the same pairs of faces and were asked to choose the face in each pair that they considered more trustworthy and to indicate the strength of this preference by choosing from the options “much more trustworthy,” “more trustworthy,” “somewhat more trustworthy,” and “slightly more trustworthy.” Pairs of faces were presented in a random order, and the side of the screen on which any particular image was shown was also randomized. Following this preadaptation test phase, participants viewed a slideshow of 15 face images where each face was shown twice and for 2 s on each occasion. Participants were instructed to “watch the faces and try to remember them.” Twenty-eight of the participants viewed masculinized versions of the face images, and the remainder viewed feminized versions of the same face images. After this adaptation phase, participants repeated the preadaptation test. Twelve participants viewed feminized faces in the adaptation phase and made attractiveness judgements, 15 participants viewed feminized faces in the adaptation phase and made trustworthiness judgements, 14 participants viewed masculinized faces in the adaptation phase and made attractiveness judgements, and 14 participants viewed masculinized faces in the adaptation phase and made trustworthiness judgements.

2.4. Initial processing of data

For participants who made attractiveness judgments in the pre- and postadaptation phases of the experiment, responses were recoded using the following eight-point scale (with high scores indicating a strong preference for the type of face seen in the adaptation phase and low scores indicating a relatively weak preference for the type of face seen in the adaptation phase):


0=The type of face that was not viewed in the adaptation phase (i.e., masculine or feminine) was rated much more attractive than the type of face viewed in the adaptation phase.

1=The type of face that was not viewed in the adaptation phase (i.e., masculine or feminine) was rated more attractive than the type of face viewed in the adaptation phase.

2=The type of face that was not viewed in the adaptation phase (i.e., masculine or feminine) was rated somewhat more attractive than the type of face viewed in the adaptation phase.

3=The type of face that was not viewed in the adaptation phase (i.e., masculine or feminine) was rated slightly more attractive than the type of face viewed in the adaptation phase.

4=The type of face viewed in the adaptation phase (i.e., masculine or feminine) was rated slightly more attractive than the type of face that was not viewed in the adaptation phase.

5=The type of face viewed in the adaptation phase (i.e., masculine or feminine) was rated somewhat more attractive than the type of face that was not viewed in the adaptation phase.

6=The type of face viewed in the adaptation phase (i.e., masculine or feminine) was rated more attractive than the type of face that was not viewed in the adaptation phase.

7=The type of face viewed in the adaptation phase (i.e., masculine or feminine) was rated much more attractive than the type of face that was not viewed in the adaptation phase.

The mean strength of preference for the type of face viewed in the adaptation phase was calculated for each participant and calculated separately for the pre- and postadaptation tests. These values were then converted to a percentage of the maximum possible strength of preference for the type of face that was seen in the adaptation phase. Corresponding values were calculated for participants who judged the trustworthiness of faces in the pre- and postadaptation phases.

3. Results

Responses were analyzed using a mixed-design ANOVA [dependent variable: strength of preference for the type of face viewed in the adaptation phase; within-subject factor: phase (preadaptation, postadaptation); between-subject factors: sex of the participant (male, female), judgement made by the participant (attractiveness, trustworthiness), type of face shown in adaptation phase (masculine, feminine)]. This analysis revealed a main effect of phase (F=5.52, df=1,47, p=.023). Preferences for the type of face viewed in the adaptation phase were stronger in the postadaptation test phase (mean percentage of the maximum possible strength of preference for the type of face that was seen in the adaptation phase=52.9, S.E.=1.6) than in the preadaptation test phase (mean=50.5, S.E.=1.6). There were no significant effects (all F<2.515, all p>.12) other than an interaction between the type of judgement made by a participant and the type of face shown in the adaptation phase (F=5.83, df=1,47, p=.020). Because this interaction did not qualify the main effect of phase we observed (i.e., was not at all related to the experimental manipulation) and was, therefore, most likely to reflect individual differences in attitudes to sexual dimorphism in male faces (see Fink and Penton-Voak, 2002 and Penton-Voak and Perrett, 2000 for a discussion of this), we did not analyze it further.

4. Discussion

Exposure to masculine or feminine male faces increased both attraction to faces of the type seen in the adaptation phase and attributions of trustworthiness to these faces relative to preadaptation phase tests. It is unsurprising that the effects of visual adaptation to faces were relatively subtle because the duration of the adaptation period was short and the duration of adaptation is positively and logarithmically related to the magnitude of face aftereffects (Leopold et al., 2005). Thus, effects of adaptation on face perceptions that occur as a consequence of longer real-life social interactions are likely to be considerably more pronounced. The effect of adaptation to masculine and feminine faces on subsequent preferences for sexual dimorphism in male faces complements attractiveness aftereffects observed for adaptation to sexual dimorphism of 2D shape (Little et al., 2005), identity (Little et al., 2005), and expanded or compressed features (Rhodes et al., 2003) and is consistent with the proposal that face preferences are recalibrated according to recent visual experience with faces (Little et al., 2005, Rhodes et al., 2003). The effect of adaptation to masculine and feminine faces on subsequent attributions of trustworthiness, however, demonstrates that face aftereffects also influence attributions of trustworthiness and (potentially) attributions of other personality characteristics that are also important for social interactions and mate choice. Furthermore, that visual adaptation to faces elicits identical effects on attributions of trustworthiness, and attraction to novel similar faces supports the proposal that visual exposure alone cannot explain the context specificity of attitudes to self-resembling faces (see DeBruine, 2005). While DeBruine (2005) demonstrated that self-resembling faces were perceived as more trustworthy, but less attractive, than other-resembling faces, we show, in this study, that adaptation to faces produces equivalent effects for attraction and trustworthiness judgments. This supports DeBruine's proposal that attitudes to self-resembling faces are not simply a by-product of visual adaptation (DeBruine, 2004).

While previous studies of variations in attitudes to masculine faces have emphasized the importance of hormone-mediated attraction to masculinity in male faces (Jones et al., 2005, Penton-Voak et al., 1999) and attributions of male-sex typical personality characteristics (Macrae, Alnwick, Milne, & Schloerscheidt, 2002), here we emphasize the role of recent visual experience with masculine and feminine faces in shaping these preferences and attitudes. It has previously been noted that adaptation effects are likely to play an important role in the development of face preferences within individuals (Little et al., 2005, Rhodes et al., 2003) and may be the proximate mechanism that underpins phenomena such as attraction to parental characteristics (e.g., imprinting-like effects; Little et al., 2003, Perrett et al., 2002) and condition-dependent face preferences (e.g., matching for attractiveness in face preference tests where there is no possibility of actual rejection; Jones et al., 2005, Little et al., 2001). In other words, positive regard for parental characteristics may be a consequence of individual differences in the type of faces individuals are adapted to (e.g., individuals with older parents are likely to have more visual experience of older faces than individuals with relatively younger parents will, Perrett et al., 2002), and matching for attractiveness on face preference tests may be a consequence of attractive individuals' being more likely to associate with other attractive individuals than are relatively unattractive individuals (see Feingold, 1988 for evidence of this). Furthermore, our findings indicate that adaptation to faces also influences the development of social stereotypes within individuals that are also important for partner and associate choices. For example, while Perrett et al. (1998) reported that masculine faces are generally perceived to be less trustworthy than feminine faces, the effect of adaptation to faces on perceptions of trustworthiness that was observed in the current study indicates that the extent to which masculine faces are perceived as untrustworthy is, to some extent, dependent on recent visual experience with masculine faces.

Acknowledgments

This research was carried out in the Face Research Laboratory at the University of Aberdeen and was supervised by BCJ and LMD.

References

DeBruine, 2004 1.DeBruine LM. Facial resemblance increases the attractiveness of same-sex faces more than other-sex faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B. 2004;271:2085–2090. MEDLINE | CrossRef

DeBruine, 2005 2.DeBruine LM. Trustworthy but not lust-worthy: Context-specific effects of facial resemblance. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B. 2005;272:919–922. MEDLINE | CrossRef

DeBruine et al., 2006 3.DeBruine LM, Jones BC, Little AC, Boothroyd LG, Perrett DI, Penton-Voak IS, et al. Correlated preferences for facial masculinity and ideal or actual partner's masculinity. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B. 2006;[Epub ahead of press; dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2005.3445].

DeBruine et al., 2005 4.DeBruine LM, Jones BC, Perrett DI. Women's attractiveness judgments of self-resembling faces change across the menstrual cycle. Hormones and Behavior. 2005;47:379–383. CrossRef

Feingold, 1988 5.Feingold A. Matching for attractiveness in romantic partners and same-sex friends: A meta-analysis and theoretical critique. Psychological Bulletin. 1988;104:226–235. CrossRef

Fink & Penton-Voak, 2002 6.Fink B, Penton-Voak IS. Evolutionary psychology of facial attractiveness. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2002;11:154–158.

Johnston et al., 2001 7.Johnston VS, Hagel R, Franklin M, Fink B, Grammer K. Male facial attractiveness: Evidence for a hormone mediated adaptive design. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2001;22:251–267.

Jones et al., 2005 8.Jones BC, Little AC, Boothroyd LG, DeBruine LM, Feinberg DR, Law Smith MJ, et al. Commitment to relationships and preferences for femininity and apparent health in faces are strongest on days of the menstrual cycle when progesterone level is high. Hormones and Behavior. 2005;48(3):283–290. CrossRef

Jones et al., 2005 9.Jones BC, Little AC, Boothroyd LG, Feinberg DR, Cornwell RE, DeBruine LM, et al. Women's physical and psychological condition independently predict their preference for apparent health in faces. Evolution and Human. 2005;26.

Jones et al., 2005 10.Jones BC, Perrett DI, Little AC, Boothroyd L, Cornwell RE, Feinberg DR, et al. Menstrual cycle, pregnancy and oral contraceptive use alter attraction to apparent health in faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B. 2005;272:347–354. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Langlois et al., 2000 11.Langlois JH, Kalakanis L, Rubenstein AJ, Larson A, Hallam M, Smoot M. Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126:390–423. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Leopold et al., 2001 12.Leopold DA, O'Toole AJ, Vetter T, Blanz V. Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealed by high-level aftereffects. Nature Neuroscience. 2001;4:89–94. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Leopold et al., 2005 13.Leopold DA, Rhodes G, Müller KM, Jeffery L. The dynamics of visual adaptation to faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B. 2005;272:897–904. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Little et al., 2001 14.Little AC, Burt DM, Penton-Voak IS, Perrett DI. Self perceived attractiveness influences human preferences for sexual dimorphism and symmetry in male faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B. 2001;268:39–44. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Little et al., 2005 15.Little AC, DeBruine LM, Jones BC. Sex-contingent face aftereffects suggest distinct neural populations code male and female faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B. 2005;272:1–5. CrossRef

Little et al., 2002 16.Little AC, Jones BC, Penton-Voak IS, Burt DM, Perrett DI. Partnership status and the temporal context of relationships influence human female preferences for sexual dimorphism in male face shape. Proceedings of the Royal Society London, Series B. 2002;269:1095–1100.

Little et al., 2003 17.Little AC, Penton-Voak IS, Burt DM, Perrett DI. Investigating an imprinting-like phenomenon in humans: Partners and opposite-sex parents have similar hair and eye colour. Evolution and Human Behaviour. 2003;24:43–51.

Loffler et al., 2005 18.Loffler G, Yourganov G, Wilkinson F, Wilson HR. fMRI evidence for the neural representation of faces. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1386–1391. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Macrae et al., 2002 19.Macrae CN, Alnwick KA, Milne AB, Schloerscheidt AM. Person perception across the menstrual cycle: Hormonal influences on social cognitive functioning. Psychological Science. 2002;13:532–536. MEDLINE

Penton-Voak & Perrett, 2000 20.Penton-Voak IS, Perrett DI. Female preference for male faces changes cyclically—Further evidence. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2000;21:39–48.

Penton-Voak et al., 1999 21.Penton-Voak IS, Perrett DI, Castles DL, Kobayashi T, Burt DM, Murray LK, et al. Menstrual cycle alters face preference. Nature. 1999;399:741–742. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Perrett et al., 1998 22.Perrett DI, Lee KJ, Penton-Voak IS, Rowland DR, Yoshikawa S, Burt DM, et al. Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature. 1998;394:884–887. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Perrett et al., 2002 23.Perrett DI, Penton-Voak IS, Little AC, Tiddeman BP, Burt DM, Oxley R, et al. Facial attractiveness judgements reflect learning of parental age characteristics. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B. 2002;269:873–880. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Rhodes et al., 2003 24.Rhodes G, Jeffery L, Watson TL, Clifford CWG, Nakayama K. Fitting the mind to the world: Face adaptation and attractiveness aftereffects. Psychological Science. 2003;14:558–566. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Rhodes et al., 2004 25.Rhodes G, Jeffery L, Watson TL, Jaquet E, Winkler C, Clifford CWG. Orientation-contingent face aftereffects and implications for face-coding mechanisms. Current Biology. 2004;14:2119–2123. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Rowland & Perrett, 1995 26.Rowland DA, Perrett DI. Manipulating facial appearance through shape and color. IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications. 1995;15:70–76. CrossRef

Tiddeman et al., 2001 27.Tiddeman BP, Burt DM, Perrett DI. Prototyping and transforming facial textures for perception research. IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications. 2001;21:42–50. CrossRef

Todorov et al., 2005 28.Todorov A, Mandisodza AN, Goren A, Hall CC. Inferences of competence from faces predict election outcomes. Science. 2005;308:1623–1626. CrossRef

Webster et al., 2004 29.Webster MA, Kaping D, Mizokami Y, Duhamel P. Adaptation to natural face categories. Nature. 2004;428:557–561. CrossRef

Winston et al., 2004 30.Winston JS, Henson RNA, Fine-Goulden MR, Dolan RJ. fMRI-adaptation reveals dissociable neural representations of identity and expression in face perception. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2004;92:1830–1839. MEDLINE | CrossRef

a School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen, Scotland, UK, AB24 3FX

b School of Psychology, University of St Andrews, Scotland, UK, KY16 9AJ

c School of Biology, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, UK, L69 7Z8

d School of Computer Science, University of St Andrews, Scotland, UK, KY16 9AJ

Corresponding author. Face Research Laboratory, School of Psychology, William Guild Building, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen, AB24 2UB, Scotland, UK. Tel.: +44 1224 273933; fax: +44 1224 273426.

 LMD was supported by a grant from the U.S. National Science Foundation (0401951), ACL was supported by a Royal Society University Research Fellowship, and GB was supported by a studentship from the University of Aberdeen 6th Century.

PII: S1090-5138(06)00017-1

doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.03.001



2007:12:08