Поиск по сайту




Пишите нам: info@ethology.ru

Follow etholog on Twitter

Система Orphus

Новости
Библиотека
Видео
Разное
Кросс-культурный метод
Старые форумы
Рекомендуем
Не в тему

список статей


Sex differences in mate choice criteria are reflected in folktales from around the world and in historical European literature

Jonathan Gottschall, Johanna Martin, Hadley Quish, Jon Rea

1. Introduction

2. Data and methods

2.1. Study 1: folktale protocol

2.2. Study 2: Western literature protocol

2.3. Assessing intercoder reliability

3. Results and discussion

Appendix. Subsamples and Cultures

References

Copyright

1. Introduction

The present article represents an attempt to extend some of the mate preference findings of Buss's (1989a) cross-cultural study with a large data set from non-Western, preindustrial populations. One criticism of the cross-cultural study by Buss and colleagues was that a preponderance of data was gathered from Western or westernized societies and, as a result, it was not possible to rule out the conclusion that regularities in mate preferences resulted from cultural transfer rather than from evolved sexual psychology Borgia, 1989, Dickemann, 1989. Many studies have since been conducted that are consistent with Buss's original findings (Baize & Schroeder, 1995, Basu & Bajyasri, 2001, Buss & Schmitt, 1993, Chuang, 2002, Cramer et al., 1996, Feingold, 1990, Feingold, 1992, Kasser & Sharma, 1999, Jensen-Campbell et al., 1995, Regan et al., 2000, Sprecher et al., 1994, Walter, 1997, Wiederman, 1993, Wiederman & Allgeier, 1992, Waynforth & Dunbar, 1995; but see Luszyk, 2001)but virtually all of these studies were conducted in western populations or in modern non-western populations. By including information derived from band, tribal, and preindustrial state populations, the current study attempts to address concerns based on the possibility of cross-cultural transfer.

The findings of this study are based on multiple-coder content analyses of the mate preferences of characters in two different sources of literary data: 658 traditional folktales from 48 culture areas, and plot and character summaries from 240 works taken to be representative of classic Western literature. While Buss (1989a) includes information on 18 different mate preference variables, we collected data for just four: preference for physical attractiveness, wealth, status, and kindness. Moreover, while Buss asked respondents to rate each variable on a 4-point scale (with 0 representing irrelevant/unimportant and 3 representing indispensable), coders in our study were asked to judge which of the four mate preference variables seemed most important to the character being coded. In all, results are based on analysis of 898 distinct folktales and works of Western literature and the mate preferences of 833 characters, 434 male and 399 female. The results of this analysis reveal cross-cultural regularities in the substance of male and female mate preferences that are consistent with the results of Buss.

2. Data and methods

2.1. Study 1: folktale protocol

Folktale collections were chosen so as to maximize the geographical variability of the sample as well as variability in levels of cultural complexity. Specialized collections focusing on specific themes, plots, or character types (e.g., Hopi Trickster Tales, Hero Tales of the South Slavs) were rejected in favor of generic collections (Hopi Folktales, Traditional Tales of the South Slavs). All collections were of traditional tales, originally transmitted through the oral tradition. In all, the study includes tales from 48 different culture areas from all inhabited continents, varying widely in ecology, geographic location, racial and ethnic composition, political systems, religious beliefs, and levels of cultural complexity. All non-English tales had been translated into English, and the sample ran the gamut from polished fairy tales to literal transcriptions of tales told in traditional contexts. A list of the collections and tales coded can be accessed via the Internet at the following URL: http://www.science.mcmaster.ca/psychology/ehb/Gottschall(FolkTales).pdf.

We chose the folktale genre for our cross-cultural sample for several reasons. First, folktales are relatively short and allow for the processing of a large sample in a relatively brief span of time. Second, we hoped to, insofar as possible, diminish problems associated with cross-cultural borrowing and contamination. While the folktales in our sample were mostly compiled, edited, and translated by Westerners (and thus may reflect Western biases), the tales were originally composed and circulated in traditional societies and can be cautiously assumed to reflect the traditional attitudes and life ways of the populations that produced them. For the purposes of this study, we assume psychological equivalence between literary characters and real human beings. In fact, the relationship between the representation of behavior and psychology in literary works and behavior and psychology in real life is a subject that awaits systematic exploration. But, for the present, it seems safe to provisionally adopt the traditional assumption of literary scholars that literary works reflect both general aspects of human nature and particular aspects of given cultures, despite the fact that they also reflect the personal idiosyncrasies of writers and tellers (for discussion of this issue, see Carroll, in press).

Once suitable collections of tales were identified, each data collector (10 female and 5 male undergraduates at St. Lawrence University) was assigned a random selection of tales from each of three culture areas. (Several researchers were responsible for tales from four culture areas. They volunteered to code collections of tales that only arrived through interlibrary loan after the main portion of the study had been completed.) The coders were participants in a seminar on research methods, focusing on content analysis methodology in literary studies. The coders were made aware that controversies existed concerning constructivist versus evolutionary accounts of human mate preferences and were told that the results of their research—whether the results predominantly suggested cross-cultural regularity or extreme cross-cultural flux—would help to resolve these controversies.

Information was gathered on the main male and female protagonists and antagonists in each tale. For the purposes of our study, a main protagonist was defined as a character who plays a central role in the action and for whom the audience is led to root predominantly for rather than predominantly against. A main antagonist was defined as a character who plays a central role in the action, who acts as an obstacle to the goals of the protagonist(s), and for whom the audience is led to root predominantly against rather than predominantly for. A maximum of one coding form was filled out for each main male and female protagonist and antagonist per story. In the event that there were, for instance, two main female protagonists, coders focused only on the female protagonist who was judged most important and prominent. Thus, between one and four coding forms were completed for each work in the sample.

For all main characters, coders answered the following question: What single feature seems most important to the character in assessing the desirability of an existing or potential mate: (1) kindness, (2) possession of wealth and/or other material resources, (3) high social status, (4) physical attractiveness, (5) other, (6) impossible to answer? In the reporting of the results that follows, however, categories two and three are collapsed into one category. In all, information on mate preferences was available for 524 folktale characters, 246 male and 278 female. These figures do not include the relatively rare instances (8% of the characters in the two studies) where coders entered “other” for a character's mate preference.

Data analysis is reported for the sample as a whole and for each of six geographical regions: the circum-Mediterranean, East Eurasia, North America, South America, Africa, and the Insular Pacific combined with Southeast Asia and the Pacific Rim (see Appendix). Where possible our division of culture areas into geographical regions follows Murdock, 1957, Murdock, 1981, although it diverges in two significant ways. First, the analysis makes up for a shortfall of tales in Murdock's Insular Pacific region and a glut of tales in his East Eurasian region by grouping several culture areas in Southeast Asia and the Pacific Rim with tales from the Insular Pacific. Second, East African tales were grouped with Africa rather than with circum-Mediterranean tales. These divergences from Murdock result from the desirability of establishing regional samples of roughly equivalent size. The sample was also divided into two broad levels of cultural complexity. The first level consists of tales that circulated primarily in unassimilated band and tribal societies, though the tales may have only been written down after assimilation. The second level consists of tales that, while they may have originated in nonstate societies, circulated for long periods in preindustrial state societies. Because the line between these categories can obviously be fine, tales from culture areas that could not be confidently placed in one of these two categories were excluded from the analysis (see Appendix for description of the two subsamples).

2.2. Study 2: Western literature protocol

Study 2 was conducted in a different semester with different student coders (7 male and 5 female), but coding procedures were identical to Study 1. Our sources of data for Western literature were a random selection of Masterplots (1977) plot summaries; the summaries were read conjointly with complementary character summaries from the Cyclopedia of Literary Characters (1995). Masterplots is a collection of roughly 1200–1500 word plot summaries of the most prominent works in Western literature. The Masterplots summaries were complemented with character summaries from the Cyclopedia of Literary Characters. The Cyclopedia provided more in-depth information on individual characters with longer summaries (150–200 words) for the main characters. Thus, for every work of Western literature coders were in possession of a Masterplots summary as well as complementary information from the Cyclopedia of Literary Characters. Each coder was responsible for 20 randomly selected sets of plot/character summaries. While Masterplots includes a small percentage of nonfictional works (e.g., summaries of historical and philosophical works), all nonfiction was excluded from our analysis. From the 240 sets of summaries, information was available for the mate preferences of 309 characters, 188 male and 121 female. A list of all characters and works coded can be found at the URL address: http://www.science.mcmaster.ca/psychology/ehb/Gottschall(EurLit).pdf.

We chose to focus on summaries of Western literature rather than the works themselves for several reasons. First, while the constitutions of literary canons are in constant flux and highly controverted, Masterplots and the Cyclopedia represent something resembling a consensus on what works form the core of the Western literary tradition. Second, and most importantly, using summaries allowed us to derive basic information from a large number of literary works in a reasonable amount of time. Of course, using summaries also comes with costs. Foremost among these is the loss of valuable information that occurs whenever a long, complex literary work is reduced to a 1500-word gloss, or a complicated character is reduced to a 200-word summary. It is doubtlessly true that many of the summaries we read omitted details relevant to the mate preferences of characters. It is also doubtlessly true that information in the summaries was colored by the personal attitudes and prejudices of their authors, despite attempts to maintain objectivity. However, it should be noted that the Masterplots and Cyclopedia summaries are not the idiosyncratic products of one individual's vision: The choice of works in these references was determined collaboratively and the actual summaries were contributed by scores of English faculty from dozens of universities (see Masterplots, v–vii). In sharing the substantial task of choosing the works and authoring the summaries, the editors of these references guarded against the possibility that the biases of individual authors would compromise the reliability of the references as a whole.

2.3. Assessing intercoder reliability

As the wording of the coding question makes clear, coders were asked to make subjective judgments about which mate preference trait seemed most important to the characters being coded. Coder subjectivity bias is the main methodological obstacle in human coder content analysis. To diminish the scope for subjectivity bias we adopted some standard safeguards: We developed a coding question that was as uncomplicated as possible, we devised simple coding instructions, and we prepared a coding dictionary with simple definitions of all potentially ambiguous terms. For the four traits coded there were no significant differences between the codings produced by male and female coders.

Intercoder reliability was also assessed in two formal tests. Test 1 assessed the reliability of the folktale coders (Study 1) in coding an assortment of 11 culturally diverse folktales; intercoder reliability was 88%. Test 2 assessed the reliability of the Western literature coders (Study 2) in coding five randomly selected sets of plot and character summaries from Masterplots and the Cyclopedia, respectively; intercoder reliability was 74%. This latter result is at the lower end of the spectrum of an acceptable estimate of reliability for content analysis and therefore results from the Western literature study must be interpreted cautiously. Many content analysis practitioners strive for reliability rates of 80% or better and consider 70% to be the minimum level of adequacy Krippendorff, 1980, Neuendorf, 2002, Weber, 1990. Establishing reliability ratings prior to actual coding of the texts, as was done in the present study, rather than having multiple coders read and code all or some fraction of the different works, is well established in content analysis research. The advantage of this approach is that it facilitates the compilation of a large data set, though it does so at the cost of some degree of precision in assessing intercoder reliability for the coded data (for discussion of different methods of reliability testing see Krippendorff, 1980, Neuendorf, 2002, Weber, 1990).

3. Results and discussion

The results of this analysis are broadly consistent with the findings of Buss (1989a). Across the folktale data, across the two levels of cultural complexity, and in the Western literature sample, male characters were significantly more likely than females to be portrayed as prizing physical attractiveness as their main criterion in mate selection (see Table 1). In the folktale sample, overall, male characters were almost 2.5 times more likely than females to place primary emphasis on physical attractiveness, while in the Western literature sample male characters were nearly four times more likely to be portrayed as placing primary emphasis on physical attractiveness. These results were significant across all subsamples except South America, where the figures still pointed in the same direction.

Table 1.

Percentage of male and female characters identified as placing primary emphasis on given mate preference criteria in samples of folk tales and classic Western literature

Physical attractiveness Wealth/status Kindness
Male % (N) Female% (N) Z score Male Female Z score Male Female Z score
Overall folk tales 56 (246) 23 (278) 7.78** 9 26 −5.03** 35 51 −3.51**
Regions South America 65 (48) 51 (39) 1.26 13 23 −1.28 22 26 −0.29
Circum-Mediterranean 42 (68) 15 (77) 3.68** 6 15 −1.68 52 70 −2.28*
East Eurasia 58 (47) 24 (59) 3.71** 11 27 −2.25* 30 49 −2.08*
Africa 63 (16) 35 (20) 1.96* 19 40 −1.45 18 25 −0.3
Insular Pacific, etc. 84 (19) 13 (24) 6.67** 5 38 −2.90** 11 50 −3.18**
Cultural complexity Bands/Tribes 67 (114) 33 (94) 5.01** 9 34 −4.47** 25 33 −1.89
Preindustrial states 50 (115) 17 (140) 5.74** 8 21 −3.18** 43 61 −3.05**
Western literature 42 (188) 11 (121) 6.84** 21 31 −2.07* 37 58 −3.61**

N is number of story characters.

See Appendix for listing of cultural groups in each region and in each level of cultural complexity).

**

P<.01.

*

P<.05.

Generally speaking, female characters more than male characters were portrayed as placing greater emphasis on a potential mate's wealth and/or social status. In the folktale sample, overall, females were almost three times as likely as males to be identified as placing primary emphasis on wealth and or social status. In the Western literature, sample characters of both sexes were coded as placing relatively higher premiums on wealth and/or status. However, female characters were only about 50% more likely than males to be identified as placing primary emphasis on wealth and/or social status. Greater female emphasis on wealth/status was invariant across the subsamples, but it was not significant in the South American, circum-Mediterranean, or African subsamples.

Also consistent with Buss (1989a), both male and female characters tended to place a premium on the kindness of potential mates. However, across 6 of the 10 comparisons female characters' emphasis on kindness was rated significantly higher than that of males.

In summary, while there was substantial variation across subsamples in the percentage of characters coded as prioritizing one of the three mate preference criteria, and while in several cases results did not reach the level of statistical significance, in no case were the general trends of sexually differentiated mate preferences violated. Male–female differences were strongest for physical attractiveness and wealth/status. Sex differences were smaller, but still suggestive, for kindness. These findings indicate cross-regional trends in human mate preferences that are consistent with Buss's (1989a) original cross-cultural study. Some of the variation in the data is likely the result of random drift in relatively small subsamples. However, this variation is also likely to indicate the importance of local physical and social environments in influencing the mate preferences of populations. While the emphasis of this study, like that of Buss, was on the regularities, identifying the specific sources of variance across geographical regions is just as important and it should draw the attention of future researchers.

Finally, the use of folktale data adds a valuable dimension to the original findings of Buss (1989a). As acknowledged in the paper itself, the Buss study was based mainly on data gathered from “urbanized, cash-economy cultures” (p. 13). Borgia (1989) criticized Buss for the dearth of aboriginal cultures in the sample and for the fact that 27 of the 32 societies were European or had “a predominantly European influence” (Buss, 1989a, p. 16). Thus, Borgia concludes: “Buss has failed to do what is necessary in this type of comparison: offer convincing evidence that the observed similarity in cross-cultural patterns of mate preference is due to convergent evolution and not to cultural transfer” (p. 16; see also Dickemann, 1989; for a response to Borgia and Dickemann, see Buss, 1989b). The findings of the current study, of sex differences in mate preferences across regions of the world and levels of cultural complexity, help to address concerns based on cross-cultural transfer.

The same would be true for other questions pursued by evolutionary researchers that require access to cross-cultural information. Samples of traditional, originally oral narratives can provide evolutionary researchers access to aspects of the lives and ways of the traditional populations they are most interested in studying and upon whom they have the most difficulty gathering relatively uncontaminated data. As the last extant band and tribal populations are assimilated into modern societies, collections of original oral tales persist, representing priceless echoes of behavior, psychology, and culture in premodern populations. While collections of originally oral tales are not pristine, having suffered potential distortion through poor translation and the whims of collectors and editors, it can be argued that they provide a different, and perhaps more direct perspective, on life in traditional societies than the heavily mediated ethnographical and anthropological accounts, or self-report data, on which much evolutionary research currently depends (see Gottschall et al., in press). This is not to suggest that traditional folktales are superior to other sources of data; it is only to suggest that they are extremely valuable and relatively neglected.

The neglect of folktales and other literary data is a result, in part, of the complications involved in attempting to extract relatively unbiased data from complex literary works. It has long been cliché to say that literary works are rich sources of information about human nature, but how does one access the information in a way that is suitable for a scientific study? Faced with what we call “the problem of access,” the systematic mining of literary data has rarely been a regular part of research in the human sciences Laszlo & Cupchick, 2003, Schram & Steen, 2001. And of those human scientists who have turned to literature (e.g., Campbell, 1949, Daly & Wilson, 1998, Freud, 1980, Jung, 1930, Raglan, 1936, Rank, 1909, Salmon & Symons, 2001), most have relied on the subjective and qualitative methods of literary studies rather than on scientific quantification (for discussion of exceptions, see Laszlo & Cupchick, 2003; Schram & Steen, 2001).

Content analysis has the potential to help solve the problem of access. It can provide access to literary information about human nature, how it manifests species typically and in specific cultural ecologies, in a way that meets the standards of the scientific method. However, systematic content analysis has been practiced only for a little more than a half-century and, outstanding studies aside, it has not yet reached methodological maturity (for a historical overview see Neuendorf, 2002, pp. 27–46). Standards and protocol continue to take shape and they vary significantly across disciplinary boundaries; quantitative analysis of literary works has been particularly rare and the work of widely varying quality (for a partial review see Vickers, 2002, pp. 98–118). The present study reflects many of the serious methodological challenges content analysts face when attempting to reduce complicated and convoluted text messages to quantitative summarizations. For instance, there are legitimate questions that can be raised about the data sources we employed, the organization of the different culture areas into regions and levels of cultural complexity, the subjectivity of the coding question, the method of reliability assessment, and the soundness of the assumption of psychological equivalence between literary characters and real people. At the same time, however, we think this research demonstrates the potential of content analysis to open up literature—a vast expanse of information about human behavior, psychology, cognition, and culture—for systematic data mining.

Appendix. Subsamples and Cultures


1.Overall (n=658 tales): Aboriginal Australian, African American, Blackfoot, Chamacoco, China, Dena, East African Tribes, Gê, Germany, Guajiro, Gypsy (Roma), Haiti, Hawaii, Hopi, Hungary, India, Inuit, Iraq, Ireland, Iroquois, Japan, Israel, Korea, !Kung San, Maya, Navaho, New Guinea, Nigerian Tribes, Nivkalé, Norway, Palestine, Persia, Mongolia, Russia, Scotland, Siberian Indians, Sikuani, Sioux, Slovakia, Southern African Tribes, Tibet, Tlingit, Vietnam, West African Tribes, Yamana, Yanomami, Yugoslav.

2.North America (n=101 tales): Blackfoot, Dena, Hopi, Inuit, Iroquois, Maya, Navaho, Sioux, Tlingit.

3.South America (n=109 tales): Chamacoco, Gê, Guajiro, Nivkalé, Sikuani, Yamana, Yanomami.

4.Europe (n=135 tales): Germany, Gypsy (Roma), Hungary, Ireland, Norway, Scotland, Slovakia, Yugoslavia.

5.Africa (and Diaspora) (n=91 tales): African American, East African Tribes, Haiti, !Kung San, Nigerian Tribes, Southern African Tribes, West African Tribes.

6.East Eurasia (n=144 tales): India, Israel, Iraq, Mongolia, Palestine, Persia, Russia, Siberia, China, Tibet.

7.Insular Pacific, Pacific Rim, and South East Asia (n=78 tales): Aboriginal Australia, Hawaii, Korea, Japan, New Guinea, Vietnam.

8.Bands and tribes (n=337 tales): Aboriginal Australia, Blackfoot, Dena, East African Tribes, Hawaii, Hopi, Inuit, Iroquois, !Kung San, Navaho, New Guinea, Nigerian Tribes, Siberian Indians, Sioux, Southern African Tribes, Tlingit, West African Tribes.

9.Preindustrial states (n=291 tales): Germany, Gypsy (Roma), Hungary, India, Iraq, Ireland, Japan, Israel, Korea, China, Norway, Palestine, Persia, Mongolia, Russia, Scotland, Slovakia, Tibet, Vietnam, Yugoslav.

References

Baize & Schroeder, 1995 1.Baize H, Schroeder J. Personality and mate selection in personal ads: evolutionary preferences in a public mate selection process. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality. 1995;10:517–536.

Basu & Bajyasri, 2001 2.Basu J, Bajyasri R. Friends and lovers: a study of human mate selection in India. Psychologia: an International Journal of Psychology in the Orient. 2001;44:281–291.

Borgia, 1989 3.Borgia G. Typology and human mate preferences. Response to D. Buss, 1989a, Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12: 1–49Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1989;12:16–17.

Buss, 1989a 4.Buss DM. Sex differences in human mate preferences: evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1989;12:1–49.

Buss, 1989b 5.Buss DM. Toward an evolutionary psychology of human mating. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1989;12:39–49Author's response to 27 commentators.

Buss & Schmitt, 1993 6.Buss DM, Schmitt DP. Sexual strategies theory: an evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review. 1993;100:204–232. CrossRef

Campbell, 1949 7.Campbell J. The hero with a thousand faces. New York: MJF Books; 1949;.

Carroll, in press 8.Carroll, J. (in press). Human nature and literary meaning: A theoretical model illustrated with a critique of Pride and Prejudice. In J. Gottschall & D. S. Wilson (Eds.), Literature and the human animal. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.

Chuang, 2002 9.Chuang Y. Sex differences in mate selection preference and sexual strategy: tests for evolutionary hypotheses. Chinese Journal of Psychology. 2002;44:75–93.

Cramer et al., 1996 10.Cramer R, Schaefer J, Reid S. Identifying the ideal mate: more evidence for male–female convergence. Current Psychology: developmental, Learning, Personality, Social. 1996;15:157–166.

Cyclopedia of Literary Characters, 1995 11.Cyclopedia of literary characters. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Salem Press; 1995;.

Daly & Wilson, 1998 12.Daly M, Wilson M. The truth about Cinderella. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson Publishers; 1998;.

Dickemann, 1989 13.Dickemann M. Aggregates, averages, and behavioral plasticity. Response to D. Buss, 1989, Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12: 1–49Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1989;12:19–20.

Feingold, 1990 14.Feingold A. Gender differences in effects of physical attractiveness on romantic attraction: a comparison across five research paradigms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990;59:981–993. CrossRef

Feingold, 1992 15.Feingold A. Gender differences in mate selection preferences: a test of the parental investment model. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;112:125–139. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Freud, 1980 16.Freud S. The Interpretation of Dreams. New York: Avon; 1980;.

Gottschall et al., 2004 17.Gottschall J15 coauthors. Patterns of characterization in folk tales across geographic regions and levels of cultural complexity: literature as a neglected source of quantitative data. Human Nature. 2004;15:365–382.

Jensen-Campbell et al., 1995 18.Jensen-Campbell LA, Graziano WG, West SG. Dominance, prosocial orientation, and female preferences: do nice guys finish last?. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995;68:427–440. CrossRef

Jung, 1930 19.Jung CG. Psychology and literature. In: The spirit in man, art, and literature. vol. 15:1930;p. 84–105.

Kasser & Sharma, 1999 20.Kasser T, Sharma Y. Reproductive freedom, educational equality, and females' preference for resource-acquisition characteristics in mates. Psychological Science. 1999;10:374–377.

Krippendorff, 1980 21.Krippendorff K. Content analysis: an introduction to its methodology. Beverly Hills: Sage; 1980;.

Laszlo & Cupchick, 2003 22.In:  Laszlo J,  Cupchick C editor. Psychology of literary narratives: studies of identity and conflict. Empirical Studies in the Arts. 23:2003;p. 1–4.

Luszyk, 2001 23.Luszyk D. Gender-specific mating preferences: a contribution to the discussion between evolutionary psychology and social role theory. Zeitschrift fuer Sozialpsychologie. 2001;32:95–106.

Masterplots, 1977 24.Masterplots: 2,010 plot stories and essay reviews from the world's fine literature. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Salem Press; 1977;.

Murdock, 1957 25.Murdock GP. World ethnographic sample. American Anthropologist. 1957;59:664–688.

Murdock, 1981 26.Murdock GP. Atlas of world cultures. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press; 1981;.

Neuendorf, 2002 27.Neuendorf K. The content analysis guidebook. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2002;.

Raglan, 1936 28.Raglan FRS. The hero: a study in tradition, myth, and drama. London: Meuthen; 1936;.

Rank, 1909 29.Rank O. The myth of the birth of the hero. New York: Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease Publishing; 1909;.

Regan et al., 2000 30.Regan P, Levin L, Sprecher S, Christopher S, Cate R. Partner preferences: what characteristics do men and women desire in their short-term sex partners and long-term romantic partners?. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality. 2000;12:1–17.

Salmon & Symons, 2001 31.Salmon C, Symons D. Warrior lovers: erotic fiction, evolution, and female sexuality. New York: Orion Publishing; 2001;.

Schram & Steen, 2001 32.In:  Schram D,  Steen G editor. The psychology and sociology of literature: in honor of Elrud Ibsch. Amsterdam: John Benjamins; 2001;.

Sprecher et al., 1994 33.Sprecher S, Sullivan Q, Hatfield E. Mate selection preferences: gender differences examined in a national sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;66:1074–1080. MEDLINE | CrossRef

Vickers, 2002 34.Vickers B. Shakespeare, co-author. New York: Oxford University Press; 2002;.

Walter, 1997 35.Walter A. The evolutionary psychology of mate selection in Morocco: a multivariate analysis. Human Nature. 1997;8:113–137.

Waynforth & Dunbar, 1995 36.Waynforth D, Dunbar R. Conditional mate choice strategies in humans: evidence from lonely hearts' advertisements. Behaviour. 1995;132:755–779.

Weber, 1990 37.Weber RP. Basic content analysis. 2nd ed.. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1990;.

Wiederman, 1993 38.Wiederman MW. Evolved gender differences in mate preferences: evidence from personal advertisements. Ethology and Sociobiology. 1993;14:331–351.

Wiederman & Allgeier, 1992 39.Wiederman MW, Allgeier ER. Gender differences in mate selection criteria: sociobiological or socioeconomic explanation?. Ethology and Sociobiology. 1992;13:115–124.

First Year Program, St. Lawrence University, Canton, NY 13617, USA

Corresponding author.

PII: S1090-5138(04)00007-8

doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(04)00007-8



2007:11:13